2015
DOI: 10.1093/arclin/acv031
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The NIH Toolbox Pattern Comparison Processing Speed Test: Normative Data

Abstract: The NIH Toolbox Pattern Comparison Processing Speed Test was developed to assess processing speed. While initial validation work provides preliminary support for this test in both children and adults, more work is needed to ensure dependability and generalizability. Thus, this replication study examines descriptive data (including age effects), test-retest reliability, and construct validity in n = 4,859 participants ages 3-85 years (matched to 2010 census data). Although the Pattern Comparison was not appropr… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
61
1
1

Year Published

2017
2017
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 83 publications
(73 citation statements)
references
References 73 publications
4
61
1
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Somewhat surprisingly given established links between working memory and processing speed (64), working memory was less closely related to performance on the Pattern Comparison Processing Speed Test than to performance on every cognitive task except the stop-signal, monetary incentive delay, and intratemporal cash choice tasks. Although the strength of the relationship between working memory and processing speed (r s = .17) is numerically similar to previous findings with the same tasks in 8-to-12-year-olds (r = .26; REF 65), individual differences in working memory were more strongly related to processes including executive attention and cognitive flexibility than to processing speed in the current cohort. In addition, behavioral signatures of working memory varied as a function of working memory ability, such that relationships between behavioral measures were generally strongest in children with low working memory performance, potentially due to factors such as low attention-to-task or motivation.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 89%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Somewhat surprisingly given established links between working memory and processing speed (64), working memory was less closely related to performance on the Pattern Comparison Processing Speed Test than to performance on every cognitive task except the stop-signal, monetary incentive delay, and intratemporal cash choice tasks. Although the strength of the relationship between working memory and processing speed (r s = .17) is numerically similar to previous findings with the same tasks in 8-to-12-year-olds (r = .26; REF 65), individual differences in working memory were more strongly related to processes including executive attention and cognitive flexibility than to processing speed in the current cohort. In addition, behavioral signatures of working memory varied as a function of working memory ability, such that relationships between behavioral measures were generally strongest in children with low working memory performance, potentially due to factors such as low attention-to-task or motivation.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 89%
“…processing speed (65,89). Children are shown two pictures and are asked to indicate whether they are the same or different.…”
Section: The Toolbox Pattern Comparison Processing Speed Test ® Measumentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The ABCD cognitive test battery (Luciana et al, 2018) assessed fluid reasoning, episodic memory, flexible thinking, attention, working memory, learning, mental rotation, processing speed, vocabulary comprehension, and oral reading. It included the Little Man Task (Acker & Acker, 1982), Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Task (mean total correct of trials 1-5) (Taylor, 1959), Matrix Reasoning subtest from the Wechsler Intelligence Test for Children-V (Wechsler, 2014), Picture Vocabulary Task (PPVT) (Gershon et al, 2013), Oral Reading Recognition Task (Gershon et al, 2013), Pattern Comparison Processing Speed Test (Carlozzi, Beaumont, Tulsky, & Gershon, 2015), Dimensional Change Card Sort Task (Zelazo, 2006), List Sorting Working Memory Test (Tulsky et al, 2013), Picture Sequence Memory Test (Bauer et al, 2013), and Flanker Task (Fan, McCandliss, Sommer, Raz, & Posner, 2002).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Factor loadings were 0.81, 0.80, and 0.76 respectively, and the factor accounted for 62.2% of the variance in the variables. A speed of processing variable was created based on three NIH toolbox tasks: processing speed, flanker task, and card sort task (all age-adjusted performance), similar to Carlozzi et al, 2015. Of note, the first of these three tasks is designed to be a measure of processing speed, while the latter two primarily reflect processing speed because for most subjects in the HCP dataset, accuracy is close to ceiling (Slotkin et al, 2012).…”
Section: Hcp Phenotypic Measuresmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Factor loadings were 0.81, 0.80, and 0.76 respectively, and the factor accounted for 62.2% of the variance in the variables. A speed of processing variable was created based on three NIH toolbox tasks: processing speed, flanker task, and card sort task (all age-adjusted performance), similar to Carlozzi et al [29]. Of note, for subjects with accuracy above 80% (which is nearly everyone in this sample), performance scoring on the latter two tasks is based exclusively on reaction time.…”
Section: Hcp Phenotypic Measuresmentioning
confidence: 99%