2017
DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0428
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The niche construction of cultural complexity: interactions between innovations, population size and the environment

Abstract: Niche construction is a process through which organisms alter their environments and, in doing so, influence or change the selective pressures to which they are subject. 'Cultural niche construction' refers specifically to the effect of cultural traits on the selective environments of other biological or cultural traits and may be especially important in human evolution. In addition, the relationship between population size and cultural accumulation has been the subject of extensive debate, in part because ant… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

0
79
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 63 publications
(79 citation statements)
references
References 32 publications
0
79
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Large families of models represent culture via a small number of traits, and, further, represent such traits as 'atomic' elements with no substantial interaction between them (e.g., Durham, 1991;Henrich, 2001;Kitcher, 2001;Henrich and Boyd, 2002;Rogers, 2010)-with a few notable exceptions (e.g., Enquist et al, 2011;Kolodny et al, 2015). Typically, when multiple traits are represented, they are taken to vary along a single dimension (e.g., Cavalli-Sforza and Feldman, 1981;Boyd and Richerson, 1985;Henrich, 2004), or function as an index of some other feature of interest (e.g., Fogarty and Creanza, 2017;Fogarty, 2018). While these models are all significant achievements, by idealising away multiple traits and trait interrelationships, they may be unable to represent a range of phenomena; notably those where the clustering of traits influences the downstream origination, distribution, and change in the trait pool over time.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Large families of models represent culture via a small number of traits, and, further, represent such traits as 'atomic' elements with no substantial interaction between them (e.g., Durham, 1991;Henrich, 2001;Kitcher, 2001;Henrich and Boyd, 2002;Rogers, 2010)-with a few notable exceptions (e.g., Enquist et al, 2011;Kolodny et al, 2015). Typically, when multiple traits are represented, they are taken to vary along a single dimension (e.g., Cavalli-Sforza and Feldman, 1981;Boyd and Richerson, 1985;Henrich, 2004), or function as an index of some other feature of interest (e.g., Fogarty and Creanza, 2017;Fogarty, 2018). While these models are all significant achievements, by idealising away multiple traits and trait interrelationships, they may be unable to represent a range of phenomena; notably those where the clustering of traits influences the downstream origination, distribution, and change in the trait pool over time.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Recently, several studies explored the consequences of different kinds of innovation, innovation rates, and "creativity" for cultural adaptation dynamics (and cultural complexity) (e.g. Kandler andLaland, 2009, 2013;Fogarty et al, 2015;Fogarty and Creanza, 2017;Fogarty, 2018). Lewis and Laland (2012), for instance, investigated the relative influence of transmission fidelity, novel invention, modification, and combination for the build-up of cumulative culture and found that while fidelity is key, modification and combination play a more important role than novel inventions.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Thus far, we have been considering environmental fluctuations as external changes that alter the fitnesses of the cultural traits in question. However, cultural traits can also alter their own environment, changing the selection pressures on both cultural and genetic traits, a process known as cultural niche construction (Laland et al, 2000; Odling-Smee et al, 2003; Creanza et al, 2012; Fogarty and Creanza, 2017). This is salient to our model: with the example of food-source choice as a cultural trait, we can envision that environmental factors such as rainfall or temperature can fluctuate over time, altering the fitness of the cultural practice of gathering or farming certain food sources.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One sense in which cultural evolution is fundamentally different from genetic evolution is that environmental pressures can drive the innovation of new cultural traits: humans exposed to cold weather can invent warmer clothing in response, whereas a bacterium exposed to antibiotics cannot invent a resistance mutant in direct response. Thus, cultural evolutionary patterns could differ in harsh environments compared to stable environments, particularly if the harsh environments provide challenges that can be ‘solved’ with new behaviors or technologies (Rendell et al, 2010; Smaldino et al, 2013; Fogarty et al, 2015; Fogarty and Creanza, 2017; Fogarty, 2018).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%