2011
DOI: 10.1162/jocn_a_00027
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Neural Sociometer: Brain Mechanisms Underlying State Self-esteem

Abstract: On the basis of the importance of social connection for survival, humans may have evolved a "sociometer"-a mechanism that translates perceptions of rejection or acceptance into state self-esteem. Here, we explored the neural underpinnings of the sociometer by examining whether neural regions responsive to rejection or acceptance were associated with state self-esteem. Participants underwent fMRI while viewing feedback words ("interesting," "boring") ostensibly chosen by another individual (confederate) to desc… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

15
147
1
1

Year Published

2012
2012
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 176 publications
(165 citation statements)
references
References 41 publications
15
147
1
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Moreover, greater reports of social distress (feeling rejected, meaningless) were associated with greater activity in the dACC. Since that initial study, several other studies have shown that other forms of socially painful experience, such as experiencing the threat of negative social evaluation (Eisenberger et al 2011a, Takahashi et al 2009, Wager et al 2009), viewing rejection-related images (Kross et al 2007), reliving a romantic rejection (Fisher et al 2010, Kross et al 2011, or being reminded of a lost loved one (Gündel et al 2003, Kersting et al 2009, O'Connor et al 2008, activate these neural regions as well. In addition, one study demonstrated that participants showed overlapping neural activity in both affective (dACC, AI) and sensory (PI, S2) regions in response to a physical pain task and a social pain task (Kross et al 2011).…”
Section: Neuroimaging Evidencementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Moreover, greater reports of social distress (feeling rejected, meaningless) were associated with greater activity in the dACC. Since that initial study, several other studies have shown that other forms of socially painful experience, such as experiencing the threat of negative social evaluation (Eisenberger et al 2011a, Takahashi et al 2009, Wager et al 2009), viewing rejection-related images (Kross et al 2007), reliving a romantic rejection (Fisher et al 2010, Kross et al 2011, or being reminded of a lost loved one (Gündel et al 2003, Kersting et al 2009, O'Connor et al 2008, activate these neural regions as well. In addition, one study demonstrated that participants showed overlapping neural activity in both affective (dACC, AI) and sensory (PI, S2) regions in response to a physical pain task and a social pain task (Kross et al 2011).…”
Section: Neuroimaging Evidencementioning
confidence: 99%
“…The relationship between expectancy and self-esteem is nicely illustrated in an fMRI study in which participants received feedback words that they had no possibility to control (Eisenberger, Inagaki, Muscatell, Haltom, & Leary, 2011). The words (e.g., shallow, boring, friendly) were ostensibly chosen by another individual who had listened to the participant's previously recorded interview.…”
Section: Self-efficacy Self-esteem and Regulatory Controlmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Previous studies highlighted the role of the dmPFC in the processing of one's own reputation (Amodio and Frith, 2006;Eisenberger et al, 2011;Frith and Frith, 2008;Izuma, 2012;Izuma et al, 2008Izuma et al, , 2010bKawamichi et al, 2013). Furthermore, the dmPFC has been shown to be involved in both positive (Izuma et al, 2008) and negative reputation (Eisenberger et al, 2011). Future studies are required to explore whether and how the dmPFC is associated with self-referential processes and whether the activity of the dmPFC is modulated by arousal.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A growing body of research has accumulated abundant evidence of how social information from other individuals modulates brain activity and affects human behavior (Edelson et al, 2011;Eisenberger et al, 2011;Garbarini et al, 2014;Ito et al, 2011;Izuma, 2012;Izuma and Adolphs, 2013;Izuma et al, 2008Izuma et al, , 2010aKing-Casas et al, 2005;Meshi et al, 2013;Zaki et al, 2011). For example, we are strongly influenced by the presence of others (Izuma et al, 2010a,b) and show a strong tendency to conform to others' opinions, even when those opinions are erroneous (Edelson et al, 2011).…”
Section: Q2mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation