1971
DOI: 10.2307/1127934
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Naming of Primary Colors by Children

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

0
12
0

Year Published

1985
1985
2014
2014

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 25 publications
(12 citation statements)
references
References 0 publications
0
12
0
Order By: Relevance
“…These kinds of differences (which are in opposition to a model of shared, pan-gender visual processing mechanisms) went unnoticed or were attributed to possible gender differences in socialization (e.g., Anyan & Quillian, 1971). Gender differences were not a focus of study for much of the work on the cognition and categorization of color (see Hardin & Maffi, 1997, for a review), quite simply because, heretofore, gender differences were not suspected as a differentiating factor in color perception.…”
Section: The Biological Basis Of Trichromacymentioning
confidence: 96%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…These kinds of differences (which are in opposition to a model of shared, pan-gender visual processing mechanisms) went unnoticed or were attributed to possible gender differences in socialization (e.g., Anyan & Quillian, 1971). Gender differences were not a focus of study for much of the work on the cognition and categorization of color (see Hardin & Maffi, 1997, for a review), quite simply because, heretofore, gender differences were not suspected as a differentiating factor in color perception.…”
Section: The Biological Basis Of Trichromacymentioning
confidence: 96%
“…In 5-to 6-year-old children, the ability to identify primary colors by name is significantlygreater in girls than in boys (although no significant difference is found in the same subjects for drawing geometric designs). The explanation offered is that "various constitutional and environmental factors undoubtedly influence the acquisition of color-naming ability by children," and "verbal skill and interest in colors may vary between boys and girls" (Anyan & Quillian, 1971, p. 1631. In adults, a number of studies suggest that color lexicons differ in fundamental ways across gender (Lakoff, 1975;Nowaczyk, 1982;Rich, 1977;Swaringen, Layman, & Wilson, 1978;Thomas, Curtis, & Bolton, 1978).…”
Section: The Biological Basis Of Trichromacymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In psycholinguistic tasks, which evoke responses to an array of colour stimuli, already in early childhood (2.5-6 years of age) girls identify primary colours by name better than boys do (Anyan and Quillian, 1971;Johnson, 1977). In adulthood, women have been found to be more accurate, compared to men, in ascribing colour names to colour samples (Greene and Gynther, 1995;Nowaczyk, 1982;Swaringen, Layman and Wilson, 1978); more consistent in their choice of a colour sample matching a given colour name (Chapanis, 1965); and reveal a greater colour-naming consensus (Moore, Romney and Hsia, 2002;Sayim, Jameson, Alvarado and Szeszel, 2005).…”
Section: Introduction: Gender Differences In Colour Vocabulary Coloumentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It may simply be that, as a result of divergent patterns of socialization for males and females, a greater verbal skill and interest in colours is instilled in women (Anyan & Quillian, 1971). Intriguingly, however, an underlying physiological explanation is possible for why there should be sexlinked differences in colour vision: sexual dimorphisms in the genes that encode the Xlinked cone photopigments that manifest as different frequencies of expression in men and women (see, for instance, Pardo et al, 2007).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%