2012
DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2011.12.002
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The name-pronunciation effect: Why people like Mr. Smith more than Mr. Colquhoun

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
79
1

Year Published

2015
2015
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 109 publications
(90 citation statements)
references
References 27 publications
(33 reference statements)
4
79
1
Order By: Relevance
“…For instance, people judge easy-to-pronounce name bearers more favorably than difficultto-pronounce name bearers (Laham, Koval, & Alter, 2012), more readily believe statements made by bearers of easy-to-pronounce names (Newman et al, 2014), and evaluate essays written in a legible handwriting more favorably than essays written in a less legible handwriting (Greifeneder et al, 2010). Even the effect of imagined pleasant contact with an outgroup MORE-LESS ASYMMETRY 8 member has been shown to depend on fluency experiences during the imagined contact, with high fluency reducing prejudice and low fluency potentially enhancing it (West & Bruckmüller, 2013).…”
Section: Consequences Of Cognitive Fluencymentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…For instance, people judge easy-to-pronounce name bearers more favorably than difficultto-pronounce name bearers (Laham, Koval, & Alter, 2012), more readily believe statements made by bearers of easy-to-pronounce names (Newman et al, 2014), and evaluate essays written in a legible handwriting more favorably than essays written in a less legible handwriting (Greifeneder et al, 2010). Even the effect of imagined pleasant contact with an outgroup MORE-LESS ASYMMETRY 8 member has been shown to depend on fluency experiences during the imagined contact, with high fluency reducing prejudice and low fluency potentially enhancing it (West & Bruckmüller, 2013).…”
Section: Consequences Of Cognitive Fluencymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, when judging how credible a source is, they might ask themselves how they feel about the source. If they experience a subtle negative feeling, they rate the source as less credible -not realizing that this feeling was not caused by the credibility of the source, but by the difficulty they experienced when pronouncing this person's name (Laham et al, 2012). We assume that people will similarly draw on meta-cognitive experiences of fluency or disfluency while processing 'more than' and 'less than' statements when they judge the respective statements.…”
Section: Consequences Of Cognitive Fluencymentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…For example, psychologists have shown that first names closer to those in host societies are associated with positive attitudes among host populations in general (Kang, 1971, Drury andMcCarthy, 1980) and more specifically among employers, coworkers and customers (Laham et al, 2011). Economists have shown that more common names result in better educational (Figlio, 2005) and labor market success, due to reduced 5 discrimination (e.g.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For instance, wanting to change one’s name because the original name is hard to pronounce[87] makes sense to a clinician, but wanting to be renamed Clark Kent “Because I am able to unleash supernatural powers”, will arouse alarm. Psychiatrist and philosopher, Karl Jaspers (1883-1969) wrote, “the mentally ill person surely has as much right to be illogical as the healthy one”[88], and this is worth keeping in mind, but what is more important than logic is safety.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%