2005
DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0409314102
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Müller-Lyer illusion explained by the statistics of image–source relationships

Abstract: The Mü ller-Lyer effect, the apparent difference in the length of a line as the result of its adornment with arrowheads or arrow tails, is the best known and most controversial of the classical geometrical illusions. By sampling a range-image database of natural scenes, we show that the perceptual effects elicited by the Mü llerLyer stimulus and its major variants are correctly predicted by the probability distributions of the possible physical sources underlying the relevant retinal images. These results supp… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
51
1

Year Published

2012
2012
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
4
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 83 publications
(56 citation statements)
references
References 26 publications
1
51
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Indeed, one of the more wellknown explanations of a number of size illusions, including the Müller-Lyer (arrowhead) illusion and the Ponzo illusion, was put forth by Richard Gregory (Gregory, 1963). He argued that many geometric illusions stem from an erroneous application of size constancy on the basis of depth cues (although see Howe & Purves, 2005, for an alternative explanation). Our height/width illusion may, like the Shepard (described above) and the Müller-Lyer (arrowhead) illusions, stem from the visual system's compensating for a perceived difference in depth.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Indeed, one of the more wellknown explanations of a number of size illusions, including the Müller-Lyer (arrowhead) illusion and the Ponzo illusion, was put forth by Richard Gregory (Gregory, 1963). He argued that many geometric illusions stem from an erroneous application of size constancy on the basis of depth cues (although see Howe & Purves, 2005, for an alternative explanation). Our height/width illusion may, like the Shepard (described above) and the Müller-Lyer (arrowhead) illusions, stem from the visual system's compensating for a perceived difference in depth.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Size-classification task Reversed susceptibility Sovrano, Albertazzi, and Rosa Salva (2015) 8 Xenotoca eiseni (redtail splitfin fish) Two-choice discrimination task Yes The arrows on the ends of the lines are similar to the corners on the inside and outside of a building or room, such that the two target lines are perceived as being at different distances, and thus different sizes Pressey (1972) Assimilation theory The '> <' arrows at the ends of one line make it appear longer than the line with arrows facing outward '< >' since the overall figure is longer Howe and Purves (2005) Probabilistic theory In natural scenes, '> <' arrows are more likely to indicate longer lines, suggesting that the Müller-Lyer illusion could be due to a probabilistic strategy of visual processing Ginsburg (1984) Selective filtering theory We judge visual information with lower spatial frequencies as being further away, causing the inner line of the '> <' arrow to be perceptually rescaled as longer inward pointing arrows '> <' were attached and shorter when outward pointing arrows '< >' were attached (Table 2). This potentially argues against Gregory's (1963) inappropriate constancy-scaling theory, since few of the species tested are likely to have been exposed to modern architecture.…”
Section: Mccreadymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Statistical trends in natural scenes often manifest as a perceptual bias in psychophysical studies (6)(7)(8)(9)(10)(11)(12). One perceptual bias that has been observed is that, all other things being equal, humans perceive lighter image regions as being closer.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%