Suggestibility in Legal Contexts 2012
DOI: 10.1002/9781118432907.ch2
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Misinformation Effect

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

0
7
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
9
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 25 publications
(7 citation statements)
references
References 66 publications
0
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…While omitting important crime details, defendants might be more prompted to forgetting actual crucial information of the crime later on, rather than when they come up with a self-generated version of the crime, wherein they distort or introduce new fabricated information surrounding the criminal experience (Van Oorsouw and Giesbrecht, 2008; Mangiulli et al, in press). Germane to this, research has shown that individuals are able to remember and distinguish the content of self-generated information over time (Chrobak and Zaragoza, 2008, 2012; Ackil and Zaragoza, 2011; Mangiulli et al, 2018a). Therefore, a narrowed strategy (i.e., selectively retrieving and omitting just some actual details of the crime) as compared with a broader way of feigning amnesia (i.e., distorting and fabricating new information of the event; Van Oorsouw and Merckelbach, 2006), might lead offenders to forgetting crucial information surrounding the criminal act.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…While omitting important crime details, defendants might be more prompted to forgetting actual crucial information of the crime later on, rather than when they come up with a self-generated version of the crime, wherein they distort or introduce new fabricated information surrounding the criminal experience (Van Oorsouw and Giesbrecht, 2008; Mangiulli et al, in press). Germane to this, research has shown that individuals are able to remember and distinguish the content of self-generated information over time (Chrobak and Zaragoza, 2008, 2012; Ackil and Zaragoza, 2011; Mangiulli et al, 2018a). Therefore, a narrowed strategy (i.e., selectively retrieving and omitting just some actual details of the crime) as compared with a broader way of feigning amnesia (i.e., distorting and fabricating new information of the event; Van Oorsouw and Merckelbach, 2006), might lead offenders to forgetting crucial information surrounding the criminal act.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Thus, even though feigning amnesia might mostly lead to omissions (e.g., Christianson and Bylin, 1999 ; van Oorsouw and Merckelbach, 2004 ), it is not surprising that van Oorsouw and Giesbrecht (2008) found that participants initially instructed to minimize culpability for a mock crime increased commission errors over time, as compared with genuinely responding controls. Accordingly, the act of coming up with a personal, self-generated story of the crime (i.e., simulated version of the crime) could enhance errors, but may not affect the number of correct details provided ( Chrobak and Zaragoza, 2008 , 2012 ; van Oorsouw and Giesbrecht, 2008 ; Ackil and Zaragoza, 2011 ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Significant research focus has been placed on the misinformation effect, which occurs when a witness is misled via exposure to incorrect post-event information (PEI) and is a powerful source of error (Ayers & Reder, 1998;Loftus, 2005;Zaragoza, Belli, & Payment, 2006). It is commonly found that individuals who are exposed to misleading information are more likely to recall that misinformation later compared with control participants (e.g., Chrobak & Zaragoza, 2012;Lindsay, Allen, Chan, & Dahl, 2004;Loftus, Miller, & Burns, 1978).…”
mentioning
confidence: 97%