1976
DOI: 10.1016/s0095-4470(19)31261-6
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The metaphysics of coarticulation

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
18
0

Year Published

1983
1983
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
4
3
3

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 52 publications
(18 citation statements)
references
References 3 publications
0
18
0
Order By: Relevance
“…One reason why Gibson's (1979) general theory of perception has seemed unlikely to serve as a useful basis on which to develop a theory of speech perception (see Fowler, 1986 and the following commentary) is that the phonological or phonetic primitives that a listener is presumed to recover-phonetic features, for example-are generally not considered to cause, in an unmediated way, the structuring of the acoustic speech signal. That is, features are believed to be in the mind of the talker, and not to be transparently reflected in the vocal-tract actions that do causally structure the signal (e.g., Hammarberg, 1976;Pierrehumbert, 1990;Repp, 1981). Both in the field of linguistics, however (e.g., Browman & Goldstein, 1990), and in psychology (Fowler, Rubin, Remez, & Turvey, 1980;Saltzman & Kelso, 1987), theories are under development in which phonological primitives are presumed to be gestures of the vocal tract.…”
Section: Inv Arlants Specifiers Cues 599mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One reason why Gibson's (1979) general theory of perception has seemed unlikely to serve as a useful basis on which to develop a theory of speech perception (see Fowler, 1986 and the following commentary) is that the phonological or phonetic primitives that a listener is presumed to recover-phonetic features, for example-are generally not considered to cause, in an unmediated way, the structuring of the acoustic speech signal. That is, features are believed to be in the mind of the talker, and not to be transparently reflected in the vocal-tract actions that do causally structure the signal (e.g., Hammarberg, 1976;Pierrehumbert, 1990;Repp, 1981). Both in the field of linguistics, however (e.g., Browman & Goldstein, 1990), and in psychology (Fowler, Rubin, Remez, & Turvey, 1980;Saltzman & Kelso, 1987), theories are under development in which phonological primitives are presumed to be gestures of the vocal tract.…”
Section: Inv Arlants Specifiers Cues 599mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…He suggested that the undershoot observed with lower stress levels and faster articulatory rates is a consequence of the earlier arrival of the final consonant control signal, which deflects the articulator away from its target trajectory. (See Daniloff &Hammarberg, 1973, andHammarberg, 1976, for a some-what more general assimilatory account of coarticulation. )…”
Section: Perception Of Coarticulated Segmentsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In connected speech, the production of a speech sound is affected by the articulatory features of its neighboring sounds [1][2][3][4][5]; this typically results in a different surface realization of the sound from its underlying form. The changes can either be a) assimilatory, when a neighboring sound extends its features to the coarticulated sound, e.g., a labial may change to a velar when immediately preceding another velar in Korean [6]; or b) dissimilatory, when the altered sound becomes less similar to a neighboring sound, e.g., a liquid /r/ is converted to a nonliquid /d/ before/after an adjacent liquid /l/ in Southern Bavarian German [7].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%