Our system is currently under heavy load due to increased usage. We're actively working on upgrades to improve performance. Thank you for your patience.
1988
DOI: 10.1037/0003-066x.43.8.614
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The meanings of personality predicates.

Abstract: Investigators who study human personality have come to rely on self-report instruments much more than their predecessors. However, a number of these investigators seem indifferent to the possibility that the theoretical meaning of a descriptive term for any quality is derived from its source of evidence. This article, relying on Frege's distinction between sense and referential meaning, suggests that theoretical meanings can change when the referents change and criticizes the practice of treating personality c… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
52
0

Year Published

1992
1992
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
7
2

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 67 publications
(54 citation statements)
references
References 34 publications
(36 reference statements)
2
52
0
Order By: Relevance
“…It is important to note that it is common for explicit selfreport measures, such as the RQ and ECR-R, and more implicit-based measures, such as the SCORS-G, to show only a limited relationship to one another as a result of cross-method assessment (Bornstein, 2002;McClelland, Koestner, & Weinberger, 1989). Kagan (1988) and Meyer and Archer (2001) highlight the importance of recognizing the infl uence of methods used to assess specifi ed constructs (i.e., clinician-rated versus self-attributed). They posit that the goal be to empirically clarify what each method accomplishes for clinical practice.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is important to note that it is common for explicit selfreport measures, such as the RQ and ECR-R, and more implicit-based measures, such as the SCORS-G, to show only a limited relationship to one another as a result of cross-method assessment (Bornstein, 2002;McClelland, Koestner, & Weinberger, 1989). Kagan (1988) and Meyer and Archer (2001) highlight the importance of recognizing the infl uence of methods used to assess specifi ed constructs (i.e., clinician-rated versus self-attributed). They posit that the goal be to empirically clarify what each method accomplishes for clinical practice.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Research then determines the ways in which these efficacy beliefs causally contribute to behavior (Bandura, Adams, & Beyer, 1977) and anxious arousal (Bandura, Reese, & Adams, 1982). This principle of assessment is similar to a point raised by Kagan (1988), who criticized psychologists' "tendency to use the same term for... different classes of data, as if the theoretical meaning of a term was unaffected by the form of its evidence" (p. 615). The meaning of a construct depends, in part, on what it refers to (its referential meaning).…”
Section: Keep Separate Response Systems Separatementioning
confidence: 96%
“…An examination of criteria besides self-reports would add considerably to our knowledge concerning the accuracy of interpersonal perception, especially because self-report data are likely to be biased (Cheek, 1982;Kagan, 1988;Wiggins, 1973). Of particular value would be any evidence regarding the relationship between strangers' judgments of targets observed in a naturalistic situation and a criterion variable characterized by both pragmatic utility and ecological validity -one that is used in everyday decisions about people.…”
Section: Ecologically Valid Criterion Variablementioning
confidence: 99%