2017
DOI: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.07.022
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The mate switching hypothesis

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
101
0
3

Year Published

2017
2017
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
3
2
1

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 187 publications
(108 citation statements)
references
References 40 publications
4
101
0
3
Order By: Relevance
“…If trajectories are dispersed, individuals might have long periods "between trajectories" during which they are not romantically interested in anyone (Person B). One literature that is especially relevant to the density concept is the literature on predictors of infidelity (Allen et al, 2008;Blow & Hartnett, 2005;Buss, Goetz, Duntley, Asao, & Conroy-Beam, 2017;Drigotas & Barta, 2001). These predictors include actor effects, such as being less agreeable or less conscientious (Schmitt et al, 2004), being anxiously attached (Bogaert & Sadava, 2002), and being male (Petersen & Hyde, 2010); relationship-level effects, such as being less committed (Drigotas, Safstrom, & Gentilia, 1999), less satisfied (Glass & Wright, 1985), and poorer at communicating (Allen et al, 2008); and external factors, such as having weaker ties to the partner's social network (Treas & Giesen, 2000) and having more opportunity to meet potential partners (e.g., Traeen & Stigum, 1998).…”
Section: Densitymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…If trajectories are dispersed, individuals might have long periods "between trajectories" during which they are not romantically interested in anyone (Person B). One literature that is especially relevant to the density concept is the literature on predictors of infidelity (Allen et al, 2008;Blow & Hartnett, 2005;Buss, Goetz, Duntley, Asao, & Conroy-Beam, 2017;Drigotas & Barta, 2001). These predictors include actor effects, such as being less agreeable or less conscientious (Schmitt et al, 2004), being anxiously attached (Bogaert & Sadava, 2002), and being male (Petersen & Hyde, 2010); relationship-level effects, such as being less committed (Drigotas, Safstrom, & Gentilia, 1999), less satisfied (Glass & Wright, 1985), and poorer at communicating (Allen et al, 2008); and external factors, such as having weaker ties to the partner's social network (Treas & Giesen, 2000) and having more opportunity to meet potential partners (e.g., Traeen & Stigum, 1998).…”
Section: Densitymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The strength of a person's preference for one means over another depends on the perceived instrumentality of each for the goal. In romantic relationships, partner substitution occurs when one permanently leaves a current partner for an alternative People as Means 10 partner (i.e., mate switching; Buss, Goetz, Duntley, Asao, & Conroy-Beam, 2017). Forty-three percent of men and 49% of women reported having engaged in such permanent romantic partner substitution at least once (Schmitt & Buss, 2001).…”
Section: Principle 8 Means Substitutionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Interpreting couple bonds as the outcome of the working of a 'marriage market' amounts to the assumption that the partners' mating builds on essentially opportunistic motives. The partners are thus willing to keep the couple alive insofar as, and as long as, they do not find an alternative that seems to secure a larger expected benefit net of all costs related to the dissolution of the incumbent bond [28], including those deriving from the possible existence of (and consequently from the after-separation agreements concerning) joint offspring. By the same token, from Becker's perspective, couple bonds tend to dissolve insofar as the partners become aware that their mating choices were based on partially unreliable information as to the characteristics of the other.…”
Section: The Opportunistic Couple: An Adequate Reference Model For Thmentioning
confidence: 99%