1984
DOI: 10.3758/bf03206335
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The local character of perspective organization

Abstract: The nature of perceived perspective orientation of 3-0 objects in space was examined for Necker cube-like ambiguous figures. Using adaptation paradigms, it was found that processes of perspective orientation have restricted spatial extents with estimated diameters of about 3 deg. It was possible to fatigue these processes selectively, but independence was found to be incomplete. It is concluded that perspective orientation is not one global process, but consists of multiple, locally defined processes, whose sp… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

0
21
1

Year Published

1987
1987
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 33 publications
(22 citation statements)
references
References 18 publications
0
21
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Extended viewing of an unambiguous stimulus biases subsequent perception of ambiguous versions of the same stimulus away from the adapted percept (Virsu, 1975; Harris, 1980; von Grünau et al, 1984; Nawrot and Blake, 1989, 1991). In experiment 1a, we investigated whether this effect occurs with bistable biological motion by adapting observers to biomotion figures that walked with an unambiguous heading direction, and then testing them with bistable versions of the same figures.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Extended viewing of an unambiguous stimulus biases subsequent perception of ambiguous versions of the same stimulus away from the adapted percept (Virsu, 1975; Harris, 1980; von Grünau et al, 1984; Nawrot and Blake, 1989, 1991). In experiment 1a, we investigated whether this effect occurs with bistable biological motion by adapting observers to biomotion figures that walked with an unambiguous heading direction, and then testing them with bistable versions of the same figures.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the bottom-up camp, most researchers have favored an explanation based on the operation of relatively passive, local processes of neural adaptation and recovery that influence perception in a bottom-up manner (e.g., Spitz & Lipman, 1962;von Grünau, Wiggin, & Reed, 1984). Although this view originated in the context of discredited Gestalt assumptions about underlying physiology (Kohler & Wallach, 1944), more recent formulations have related adaptation effects to the fatigue and recovery of localized neural "channels" that often are hypothesized to play a major role in the visual system (e.g., Anstis, Verstraten, & Mather, 1998;Braddick, Campbell, & Atkinson, 1978;Regan, 1982).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, selectively adapting one underlying cortical structure through extended viewing of an unambiguous version of a reversible figure increases the likelihood that the opposite organization will be perceived when an ambiguous version of the same figure is subsequently presented (e.g., Long, Toppino, & Mondin, 1992;Virsu, 1975). The localized nature of the underlying processes is revealed by the finding that the magnitude of adaptation effects declines markedly with the extent to which the adaptation and test stimuli are presented to different retinal locations (Toppino & Long, 1987;von Grünau et al, 1984).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Within the current literature, there are investigators who favor cognitive processes such as learning and problem solving, which may influence illusory reversals in a "top-down" manner (e.g., Girgus, Rock, & Egatz, 1977;Gregory, 1974;Rock, 1975). There are also investigators who favor the more passive or "bottom-up" processes of neural fatigue and recovery among cortical structures as determiners of figural reversal (e.g., Nawrot & Blake, 1989;Palmer & Bucher, 1981;von Grunau, Wiggin, & Reed, 1984). These interpretive differences do not result from any lack of empirical effort.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%