2017
DOI: 10.1038/s41598-017-11406-7
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The life-span trajectory of visual perception of 3D objects

Abstract: Deriving a 3D structural representation of an object from its 2D input is one of the great challenges for the visual system and yet, this type of representation is critical for the successful recognition of and interaction with objects. Perhaps reflecting the importance of this computation, infants have some sensitivity to 3D structural information, and this sensitivity is, at least, partially preserved in the elderly population. To map precisely the life-span trajectory of this key visual computation, in a se… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
11
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
2

Relationship

3
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 12 publications
(11 citation statements)
references
References 37 publications
0
11
0
Order By: Relevance
“…For example, while sensitivity to faces is observed even in newborns (Mondloch et al, 1999), this perceptual ability has a prolonged developmental trajectory that lasts even into adulthood (Germine, Duchaine, & Nakayama, 2011). Another instance of a coarse-to-fine grained development trajectory comes from the case of 3D perception: sensitivity to 3D information is evident even in infancy as demonstrated by the seminal visual cliff experiment (Gibson & Walk, 1960), but more refined representations are subject to a protracted developmental trajectory (Freud & Behrmann, 2017;Nishimura et al, 2015).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…For example, while sensitivity to faces is observed even in newborns (Mondloch et al, 1999), this perceptual ability has a prolonged developmental trajectory that lasts even into adulthood (Germine, Duchaine, & Nakayama, 2011). Another instance of a coarse-to-fine grained development trajectory comes from the case of 3D perception: sensitivity to 3D information is evident even in infancy as demonstrated by the seminal visual cliff experiment (Gibson & Walk, 1960), but more refined representations are subject to a protracted developmental trajectory (Freud & Behrmann, 2017;Nishimura et al, 2015).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Previous developmental investigations have provided evidence for a rapid emergence of visual representations that support perceptual behaviors (e.g., Face perception: de Heering et al, 2008;Depth perception: Soska & Johnson, 2008). However, these representations are coarse in nature, and then increasingly refined in the context of a protracted developmental trajectory (e.g., Freud & Behrmann, 2017;Hadad, Maurer, & Lewis, 2011;Kovacs, Kozma, Feher, & Benedek, 1999;Nishimura, Scherf, Zachariou, Tarr, & Behrmann, 2015).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One potential source of variability in the coherence of neural responses amongst children may be related to the ongoing developmental changes that occur in perceptual, cognitive, social, and emotional capacities and the supporting neural circuitry. For example, the perception of real-world objects (Freud and Behrmann, 2017) and the neural basis of size constancy of those objects (Nishimura et al, 2015) continues to evolve throughout childhood. The behavioral (Fry and Hale, 2000) and neural (Scherf et al, 2006) basis of working memory, which could influence the amount information children process as they observe and interpret the natural world, also undergo change.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…S1 summarizes group-level performance with respect to accuracy and reaction time, separately). Inverse efficiency has been shown to incorporate varying strategies across participants effectively (31)(32)(33). A summary of group-level responses is shown in Fig.…”
Section: Analysis and Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%