1997
DOI: 10.1098/rstb.1997.0105
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The knowledge base of the oculomotor system

Abstract: In everyday life, eye movements enable the eyes to gather the information required for motor actions. They are thus proactive, anticipating actions rather than just responding to stimuli. This means that the oculomotor system needs to know where to look and what to look for. Using examples from table tennis, driving and music reading we show that the information the eye movement system requires is very varied in origin and highly task speci¢c, and it is suggested that the control program or schema for a partic… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

19
235
2
3

Year Published

2007
2007
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
10

Relationship

0
10

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 314 publications
(261 citation statements)
references
References 20 publications
19
235
2
3
Order By: Relevance
“…Since human action is associated with a visual-motor delay (Hayhoe and Ballard, 2005;Land, 2006), a visual buffer of 0,80 to 2,00 seconds is used in locomotion (Land and Furneaux, 1997;Wilkie and Wann, 2003;Wilkie et al, 2008). Considering a visual buffer of one second, participants in our current experiment should have looked approximately 2,5 meter in front of them in the slowest condition.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Since human action is associated with a visual-motor delay (Hayhoe and Ballard, 2005;Land, 2006), a visual buffer of 0,80 to 2,00 seconds is used in locomotion (Land and Furneaux, 1997;Wilkie and Wann, 2003;Wilkie et al, 2008). Considering a visual buffer of one second, participants in our current experiment should have looked approximately 2,5 meter in front of them in the slowest condition.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Here again all 1058 trials without missing data are included. When performing everyday tasks our eyes are usually directed at the object or objects that are relevant for what we are doing at that moment Johansson et al 2001;Land et al 1999;Land and Hayhoe 2001;Triesch et al 2003), or toward positions at which critical information is expected to become available (e.g., information about how a ball bounces; Land and Furneaux 1997;Land and McLeod 2000). We could therefore tentatively conclude from Fig.…”
Section: Eye Movementsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Similarly, different action intentions direct attention differently to object features for processing (e.g., Bekkering & Neggers, 2002;Fischer & Hoellen, 2004;Symes, Tucker, Ellis, Vainio, & Ottoboni, 2008). Eye movements during visually guided actions shed further light on the close relationship between vision, action and language (Land & Furneaux, 1997;Johansson, Westling, Bäckström, & Flanagan, 2001). For example when humans interact with objects, their eyes move ahead of their hands to support the on-line control of grasping (e.g., Bekkering & Neggers, 2002).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%