1999
DOI: 10.3758/bf03210819
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The item and list methods of directed forgetting: Test differences and the role of demand characteristics

Abstract: In directed forgetting, the item method presents instructions to remember or to forget individual items; the list method presents a single mid-list instruction to forget the first half of the list. Initial free recall was better for remember (R) words than for forget (F) words under both methods. Offered 50 cents for each additional F word, subjects could recall almost no more items, eliminating a demand characteristics explanation. On a yes/no recognition test, only the item method showed directed forgetting.… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2

Citation Types

8
213
3
3

Year Published

2005
2005
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 198 publications
(230 citation statements)
references
References 19 publications
8
213
3
3
Order By: Relevance
“…As we were interested in the latter form of cognitive control, we used the list method in our study. The list method (e.g., MacLeod, 1998MacLeod, , 1999Zellner & Bäuml, 2006) assesses cognitive control either through intentional efforts to remember and forget (see also Bjork, Bjork, & Anderson, 1998) or through efforts to change one's mental context (Sahakyan, Delaney, & Goodmon, 2008;Sahakyan & Foster, 2009;Sahakyan & Kelley, 2002). More specifically, it requires an intentional effort to control what is forgotten and what is remembered: participants must voluntarily not recall TBF items and voluntarily recall the TBR items (Bjork et al, 1998;Power et al, 2000).…”
Section: Directed Forgettingmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…As we were interested in the latter form of cognitive control, we used the list method in our study. The list method (e.g., MacLeod, 1998MacLeod, , 1999Zellner & Bäuml, 2006) assesses cognitive control either through intentional efforts to remember and forget (see also Bjork, Bjork, & Anderson, 1998) or through efforts to change one's mental context (Sahakyan, Delaney, & Goodmon, 2008;Sahakyan & Foster, 2009;Sahakyan & Kelley, 2002). More specifically, it requires an intentional effort to control what is forgotten and what is remembered: participants must voluntarily not recall TBF items and voluntarily recall the TBR items (Bjork et al, 1998;Power et al, 2000).…”
Section: Directed Forgettingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Thus, it examines the ability to "control the contents of consciousness" (Whetstone, Cross, & Whetstone, 1996). Two methods are commonly used (Basden & Basden, 1996;Basden, Basden, & Gargano, 1993;MacLeod, 1998MacLeod, , 1999: (1) the item-by-item cueing method (i.e., word method), which tends to induce rehearsal mechanisms during encoding and (2) the list cueing method (i.e., list method), where "forget" instructions are provided after the first half of the list and "remember" instructions occur after the second half, which requires a specific effort to forget and/or control for context changes. As we were interested in the latter form of cognitive control, we used the list method in our study.…”
Section: Directed Forgettingmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…However, if participants tend to change to a better encoding strategy following a forget instruction, then the List 2 benefits of directed forgetting should be observed in recognition testing as well. Prior research has consistently failed to detect the benefits (and the costs) with recognition tests using relatively short lists of 12-16 items per list (Basden & Basden, 1996;Basden et al, 1993;Geiselman et al, 1983;Gross et al, 1970;MacLeod, 1999;Schmitter-Edgecombe et al, 2004;Whetstone et al, 1996; but see E. L. Bjork & Bjork, 2003). We suspected that benefits in recognition studies were too small to detect with shorter lists; therefore, we systematically explored the role of list length in directed forgetting.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%