2017
DOI: 10.1007/s11166-017-9259-x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The intrinsic value of choice: The propensity to under-delegate in the face of potential gains and losses

Abstract: Human beings are often faced with a pervasive problem: whether to make their own decision or to delegate the decision task to someone else. Here, we test whether people are inclined to forgo monetary rewards in order to retain agency when faced with choices that could lead to losses and gains. In a simple choice task, we show that participants choose to pay in order to control their own payoff more than they should if they were to maximize monetary rewards and minimize monetary losses. This tendency cannot be … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

3
28
0
1

Year Published

2019
2019
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 63 publications
(38 citation statements)
references
References 49 publications
3
28
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Perceived control has been proposed to function as an intrinsic reward (Bobadilla-Suarez et al, 2017;Deci & Ryan, 2000;Leotti & Delgado, 2011), which suggests a potential mechanism underlying the effects on memory observed in our study. Both humans and non-human animals are sensitive to the controllability of their environments (Moscarello & Hartley, 2017) and prefer having agency (Bown, Read, & Summers, 2003;Catania & Sagvolden, 1980;Cordova & Lepper, 1996;Leotti, Iyengar, & Ochsner, 2010;Suzuki, 1997;Suzuki, 1999).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 52%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Perceived control has been proposed to function as an intrinsic reward (Bobadilla-Suarez et al, 2017;Deci & Ryan, 2000;Leotti & Delgado, 2011), which suggests a potential mechanism underlying the effects on memory observed in our study. Both humans and non-human animals are sensitive to the controllability of their environments (Moscarello & Hartley, 2017) and prefer having agency (Bown, Read, & Summers, 2003;Catania & Sagvolden, 1980;Cordova & Lepper, 1996;Leotti, Iyengar, & Ochsner, 2010;Suzuki, 1997;Suzuki, 1999).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 52%
“…Both humans and non-human animals are sensitive to the controllability of their environments (Moscarello & Hartley, 2017) and prefer having agency (Bown, Read, & Summers, 2003;Catania & Sagvolden, 1980;Cordova & Lepper, 1996;Leotti, Iyengar, & Ochsner, 2010;Suzuki, 1997;Suzuki, 1999). People will pay a premium to have control even doing so is suboptimal (Bobadilla-Suarez et al, 2017;Owens, Grossman, & Fackler, 2014;. Consistent with this evidence that control is subjectively rewarding, striatal brain regions typically engaged during the anticipation and receipt of reward are also sensitive to conditions of agency (Fujiwara et al, 2013;Leotti & Delgado, 2011;Murty et al, 2015;, and rewards that are obtained contingently elicit greater striatal activity (Tricomi, Delgado, & Fiez, 2004) and function as more robust reinforcers (Cockburn, Collins, & Frank, 2014) than those that are passively received.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Based on the premise that experiencing a certain degree of choice is generally appreciated (e.g. Aoki et al, 2014;Bobadilla-Suarez, Sunstein, & Sharot, 2017;Bown, Read, & Summers, 2003;Leotti & Delgado, 2011), the experimental paradigm to measure social mindfulness (henceforth the SoMi paradigm) hinges on leaving or limiting choice to others. In a dyadic task, a first mover chooses from a set of three or four products, with the information that another person will choose second.…”
Section: Social Mindfulness and Cooperationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Subjects are, for example, unwilling to use algorithms that are far better in predicting outcomes than they are—unless they can have some small effect on the predicted result (Dietvorst, Simmons, & Massey, 2016). Likewise, they prefer to take a decision themselves when interacting with humans (Bobadilla‐Suarez, Sunstein, & Sharot, 2017; Dominguez‐Martinez, Sloof, & von Siemens, 2014; Fehr, Schmidt, Herz, & Wilkening, 2013). Neri and Rommeswinkel (2014), for example, show that subjects are interference‐averse in the sense that they prefer others not to affect their payoff.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%