2019
DOI: 10.1515/zfs-2019-2002
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The interpretation of German personal pronouns and d-pronouns

Abstract: Three experiments investigated the interpretation and production of pronouns in German. The first two experiments probed the preferred interpretation of a pronoun in contexts containing two potential antecedents by having participants complete a sentence fragment starting either with a personal pronoun or a d-pronoun. We systematically varied three properties of the potential antecedents: syntactic function, linear position, and topicality. The results confirm a subject preference for personal pronouns. The pr… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

4
28
2

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 13 publications
(37 citation statements)
references
References 29 publications
4
28
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Findings on the interpretation of er have found that this form refers preferentially to a prominent antecedent from the prior discourse (Bader & Portele, 2019;Bouma & Hopp, 2006, 2007Ellert & Holler, 2011;Schumacher et al, 2016), while it retains flexibility in its interpretation and can also refer felicitously to less prominent antecedents, in particular when multiple prominence hierarchies come into play Schumacher et al, 2016Schumacher et al, , 2017Wilson, 2009). Findings for der mainly agree that it refers to a less prominent antecedent and is more rigid in its interpretation (Bader & Portele, 2019;Kaiser, 2011b;Schumacher et al, 2015Schumacher et al, , 2016Schumacher et al, , 2017Wilson, 2009). However, there is no firm consensus across studies on the features contributing to prominence as it relates to pronoun resolution in German.…”
Section: Personal and Demonstrative Pronouns In Germanmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Findings on the interpretation of er have found that this form refers preferentially to a prominent antecedent from the prior discourse (Bader & Portele, 2019;Bouma & Hopp, 2006, 2007Ellert & Holler, 2011;Schumacher et al, 2016), while it retains flexibility in its interpretation and can also refer felicitously to less prominent antecedents, in particular when multiple prominence hierarchies come into play Schumacher et al, 2016Schumacher et al, , 2017Wilson, 2009). Findings for der mainly agree that it refers to a less prominent antecedent and is more rigid in its interpretation (Bader & Portele, 2019;Kaiser, 2011b;Schumacher et al, 2015Schumacher et al, , 2016Schumacher et al, , 2017Wilson, 2009). However, there is no firm consensus across studies on the features contributing to prominence as it relates to pronoun resolution in German.…”
Section: Personal and Demonstrative Pronouns In Germanmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Nevertheless, these studies also showed that it was not the thematic role alone that determined resolution preferences, rather, it was thematic role in combination with grammatical role and linear order that determined these preferences. Relatedly, Portele and Bader (2016) claim that a combination of syntactic prominence and givenness is a key component in differentiating the two pronouns, and Bader and Portele (2019) claim that d-pronouns favor the least prominent antecedent that can be determined by a combination of topichood, grammatical role, and linear position, while the personal pronoun in contrast appears to favor the subject of the previous clause.…”
Section: Personal and Demonstrative Pronouns In Germanmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…For clitics in German, see Zifonun et al (1997: 316-20) and Nübling (1992); for case distinction and levelling in personal pronouns across German dialects, see Howe (1996: 262-82); for the development of German pronouns from a diachronic perspective, see Howe (1996: 241-61). See Bader and Portele (2019) and Weinert (2007) From Figure 22, it can be observed that solely three personal pronouns, namely the first and second-person singular as well as the third-person singular masculine, possess a distinct form for every single case. All other pronouns either have a joint accusative-dative or nominative-accusative form (Howe 1996: 262).…”
Section: Germanmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…According to normative usage, the grammatical gender of the demonstrative pronoun in (5) needs to agree with the grammatical gender of the antecedent in Dutch (het huisN > dat, but de flesC > die), and the same holds for German and Maaslands. 10 Others use the term 'rhematic reference pronoun'(Weinrich et al 2003: 380) or 'd-pronoun'(Bader & Portele 2019).11 Importantly, while demonstratives and personal pronouns can often be used interchangeably, this is not possible in all contexts (for Dutch, see Audring 2020;Kaiser & Trueswell 2004; for German, seeBader & Portele 2019;Weinert 2007). Besides syntax, salience of the antecedent and contrast have been argued to play a role.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%