2015
DOI: 10.1037/a0039057
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The interplay of spatial attentional biases and mental codes in VSTM: Developmentally informed hypotheses.

Abstract: What cognitive processes influence how well we maintain information in visual short-term memory (VSTM)? We used a developmentally informed design to delve into the interplay of top-down spatial biases with the nature of the internal memory codes, motivated by documented changes for both factors over childhood. Seven-year-olds, 11-year-olds, and adults completed a VSTM task in which they decided whether a probe item had been present in a preceding memory array. Spatial cues guided participants' attention to the… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

2
15
0

Year Published

2015
2015
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

3
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 16 publications
(22 citation statements)
references
References 106 publications
(169 reference statements)
2
15
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In this paradigm, visual search for targets embedded in repeated scenes is more efficient compared with visual search for a novel target not associated with a contextual memory trace. Furthermore, attention orienting is most effective for memoranda for which familiar representations are available in long-term memory, both in children and in adults 98 .…”
Section: Visual Attention Development and Memorymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In this paradigm, visual search for targets embedded in repeated scenes is more efficient compared with visual search for a novel target not associated with a contextual memory trace. Furthermore, attention orienting is most effective for memoranda for which familiar representations are available in long-term memory, both in children and in adults 98 .…”
Section: Visual Attention Development and Memorymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Recent work has also started to explore the extent to which children are able to direct their attention towards aspects of a task environment that are particularly goal-relevant (e.g. Cowan, Fristoe, Elliott, Brunner, & Saults, 2006;Cowan, Morey, AuBuchon, Zwilling, & Gilchrist, 2010;Shimi & Scerif, 2015;Shimi, Nobre, Astle, & Scerif, 2014). For example, Shimi et al (2014) presented 7 year-olds, 11 year-olds and young adults with simultaneous four-object arrays, followed by a single recognition probe, while manipulating the timing (before versus after encoding) and location (central versus peripheral) of visual cues orienting participants to a particular item in the array.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…VWM increases dramatically with age (Gathercole, 1999 ; Cowan et al, 2005 ) with accompanied maturational changes in the brain (Kwon et al, 2002 ; Klingberg et al, 2002 ; Luna et al, 2004 ; Crone and Ridderinkhof, 2011 ; Jolles et al, 2011 ; Barriga-Paulino et al, 2014 ). Driven by the advances in the adult cognitive neuroscience literature and given that selective attention also undergoes dramatic improvement during childhood (Plude et al, 1994 ; Scerif, 2010 ; Johnson, 2011 ; Stevens and Bavelier, 2012 ), recent developmental research has also started examining the influence of visual attention mechanisms on the developing VWM system (Olesen et al, 2007 ; Cowan et al, 2010 ; Ross-Sheehy et al, 2011 ; Sander et al, 2011 ; Wendelken et al, 2011 ; Astle et al, 2012 , 2014 ; Markant and Amso, 2013 ; Shimi et al, 2014a , b ; Shimi and Scerif, 2015 ), rather than focusing solely on increases in VWM storage. Extending the adult findings to the developmental domain, in a recent study, Shimi et al ( 2014a ) demonstrated that age-related differences in the temporal dynamics of attentional orienting mechanisms before or after encoding items in VWM contributed to differences in VWM performance between children and adults.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%