2019
DOI: 10.1177/1747021819891068
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The interplay between non-symbolic number and its continuous visual properties revisited: Effects of mixing trials of different types

Abstract: In the last few years, the existence of a pure number sense has been challenged. Recent studies suggest that numerosity processing is influenced not only by the number of elements in a display but also by continuous magnitudes, such as the size of the elements. The aim of our study was to replicate and extend the findings by Gebuis and Reynvoet, who systematically manipulated different continuous magnitudes either congruently or incongruently with discrete numerosity. We were particularly interested in finding… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2

Citation Types

7
24
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 11 publications
(33 citation statements)
references
References 64 publications
(117 reference statements)
7
24
0
Order By: Relevance
“…This observation is consistent with the results from our model simulations, which assume an upper bound on the gain of processing available to an observer on any given trial (Li et al, 2017;Spitzer et al, 2017). It remains to be shown whether our experimental results with symbolic numbers will generalize to other input formats (e.g., using analogue sensory continua; Liu et al, 2015;Marinova et al, 2020;Pekár & Kinder, 2019;Rosenbaum et al, 2021;, where the distribution and range of input values may play an additional role. For instance, while the discrete numerical samples in our experiment were easily readable (i.e., early sensory noise was presumably negligible), other sensory-perceptual inputs may be more prone to, e.g., range adaptation effects (Brenner et al, 2000;Fairhall et al, 2001;Smirnakis et al, 1997;Wark et al, 2007), which might also impact the shape of psychometric weighting.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 87%
“…This observation is consistent with the results from our model simulations, which assume an upper bound on the gain of processing available to an observer on any given trial (Li et al, 2017;Spitzer et al, 2017). It remains to be shown whether our experimental results with symbolic numbers will generalize to other input formats (e.g., using analogue sensory continua; Liu et al, 2015;Marinova et al, 2020;Pekár & Kinder, 2019;Rosenbaum et al, 2021;, where the distribution and range of input values may play an additional role. For instance, while the discrete numerical samples in our experiment were easily readable (i.e., early sensory noise was presumably negligible), other sensory-perceptual inputs may be more prone to, e.g., range adaptation effects (Brenner et al, 2000;Fairhall et al, 2001;Smirnakis et al, 1997;Wark et al, 2007), which might also impact the shape of psychometric weighting.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 87%
“…A challenge for the design of visual dot numerosity stimuli is that one cannot change the numerosity and hold all other stimulus' parameters constant. As a consequence, stimuli can contain sensory cues, which are physical stimulus properties that are nonnumerical, such as total surface area or dot diameter (Gebuis et al, 2016; Pekár & Kinder, 2019; Piazza et al, 2004; Salti et al, 2017). The dot position in our stimuli was chosen randomly, which does not exclude minor differences in the convex hull, namely the area within a stimulus covered with dots.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…To ensure that participants draw on the approximation of numerosity, as opposed to counting or other strategies, some conceptual distinctions are made. First, humans can process numerosity in two different formats: symbolic (e.g., Arabic numbers) and nonsymbolic, the latter requires an estimation of the numerosity through the ANS (Gebuis & Reynvoet, 2012; Pekár & Kinder, 2019). Furthermore, in order to investigate number estimation, it has to be ensured that participants do not use subitizing, which is the almost immediate, precise, and effortless (i.e., without counting) recognition of set sizes of up to 4–5 items (Cohen & Henik, 2016; Kaufman, Lord, Reese, & Volkmann, 1949; Trick & Pylyshyn, 1994).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…This aspect may create experimental error variation, and thus may reduce statistical power and reproducibility of results (Chouinard-Thuly et al, 2017 ; Gerlai, 2017 ; Gerlai, 2019 ). Furthermore, precise parametric control of most features of the live stimulus fish is impossible (e.g., see Gebuis & Reynvoet, 2012 ; Pekár & Kinder, 2020 ), and thus systematic analysis of the potential effects of such factors is difficult. A powerful, yet simple, method with which these issues may be addressed is the presentation of video-recordings.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%