2016
DOI: 10.3758/s13421-016-0646-3
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The influences of valence and arousal on judgments of learning and on recall

Abstract: Much is known about how the emotional content of words affects memory for those words, but only recently have researchers begun to investigate whether emotional content influences metamemory-that is, learners' assessments of what is or is not memorable. The present study replicated recent work demonstrating that judgments of learning (JOLs) do indeed reflect the superior memorability of words with emotional content. We further contrasted two hypotheses regarding this effect: a physiological account in which em… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

6
35
2

Year Published

2017
2017
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 39 publications
(43 citation statements)
references
References 53 publications
(131 reference statements)
6
35
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Neutral words were low in arousal and neutral in valence, whereas emotional words were high in arousal and either positive or negative in valence. Hence, as has been done in prior studies, we manipulated arousal and valence jointly to maximize effects of emotionality on JOLs (Tauber & Dunlosky, 2012;Zimmerman & Kelley, 2010; but see Hourihan et al, 2017). If information from the two cues is integrated in JOLs, individual-level analyses should reveal that a large number of participants base their JOLs on both cues.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Neutral words were low in arousal and neutral in valence, whereas emotional words were high in arousal and either positive or negative in valence. Hence, as has been done in prior studies, we manipulated arousal and valence jointly to maximize effects of emotionality on JOLs (Tauber & Dunlosky, 2012;Zimmerman & Kelley, 2010; but see Hourihan et al, 2017). If information from the two cues is integrated in JOLs, individual-level analyses should reveal that a large number of participants base their JOLs on both cues.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In paired-associate learning, Begg, Duft, Lalonde, Melnick, and Sanvito (1989) found that cue concreteness and target concreteness both affected JOLs, whereas Illman and Morrison (2011) reported that JOLs were sensitive to cue imageability and target age of acquisition but not to cue age of acquisition and target imageability. Also, Hourihan, Fraundorf, and Benjamin (2017) revealed that JOLs were sensitive to word frequency but insensitive to valence and arousal.…”
Section: Cue Integration In Jolsmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…Such a specific learning experience, characterized by intentional encoding and the engagement of control processes, may be associated with activation in the prefrontal cortex (Ranganath, 2010), which, in turn, contributes to the generation of slow oscillations and thus potentially impacts hippocampal reactivations during SWS (Batterink, Creery, & Paller, 2016;Diekelmann & Born, 2010). The salience of emotional stimuli may attenuate these tagging mechanisms during encoding as the material is perceived as highly distinctive and easily memorable, seemingly requiring less effort to successfully retain the item and its source feature (see Hourihan, Fraundorf, & Benjamin, 2017, for evidence that emotional material influences metamnemonic judgments in this manner). On the other hand, neutral material may be perceived as less conspicuous, thus engaging more strategic processing mechanisms during intentional encoding.…”
Section: Source Memorymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The relation between JOLs for emotional items and performance has not been detected consistently: in some studies, emotional faces are associated with higher JOLs than neutral faces, but no differences in recognition are found (Nomi et al, 2013); in other studies, participants recall more emotional words, just as they predicted, but only in free recall and not in cued recall (Zimmerman & Kelley, 2010). Some studies suggest that arousal (not valence) biases judgments: higher JOLs are given to high-arousal items (that have neutral emotional value), even though no effect of arousal on actual recall or recognition was found (Hourihan et al, 2017).…”
Section: Studies Examining the Relationship Between Affectivity And Nmentioning
confidence: 89%
“…Judgments of learning are thought to be based not only on multiple cues, such as general beliefs about one's memory functioning and experience with similar types of tasks in the past (Hertzog, Dixon, & Hultsch, 1990;Mazzoni & Comoldi, 1993), but also on the properties of the items themselves (such as word frequency or concreteness, Koriat, 1997;Witherby & Tauber, 2017). JOLs are known to be impacted by the affective value of stimuli: they are typically higher for emotional than for neutral items (Hourihan, Fraundorf, & Benjamin, 2017;Nomi, Rhodes, & Cleary, 2013;Tauber & Dunlosky, 2012, Tauber, Dunlosky, Urry, & Opitz, 2017Witherby & Tauber, 2018;Zimmerman & Kelley, 2010). There are two popular non-exclusive explanations for this effect: the first states that the distinctiveness of emotional information serves as a cue for predicting future recall; the second states that physiological arousal mimics the feeling of fluency or familiarity (Witherby & Tauber, 2018;Zimmerman & Kelley, 2010).…”
Section: Studies Examining the Relationship Between Affectivity And Nmentioning
confidence: 99%