2018
DOI: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2018.01.007
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The influence of vision, touch, and proprioception on body representation of the lower limbs

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

20
63
2

Year Published

2018
2018
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
5
1
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 45 publications
(85 citation statements)
references
References 44 publications
20
63
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Similar to observations of size overestimation for ankles and wrists (Longo, 2017;Stone et al, 2018), the direction of distortion for the lower face follows the direction of movement. That is, the mouth and chin are perceived lower, shifted towards the body, increasing the perceived length of this region.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 75%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Similar to observations of size overestimation for ankles and wrists (Longo, 2017;Stone et al, 2018), the direction of distortion for the lower face follows the direction of movement. That is, the mouth and chin are perceived lower, shifted towards the body, increasing the perceived length of this region.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 75%
“…Furthermore, the face is normally seen in movement (Tsakiris, 2008), and the stored image of the face may include details of possible movement and its layout, as it occurs for other body parts, such as the hands (Bremner, Holmes, & Spence, 2008) and lower limbs (Stone et al, 2018). To explore this, we analysed the shift of locational responses, which may also explain the direction of the distortions.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…For instance, Longo and Haggard (2011) 31 found a bias to overestimate the distance between touches oriented with the medio-lateral 32 hand axis compared to the proximo-distal axis. Similar anisotropies have been reported on 33 several other body parts, including the forearm (Green, 1982;Le Cornu Knight et al, 2014;34 Marks et al, 1982), thigh (Green, 1982), shin (Stone, Keizer, & Dijkerman, 2018), and face 35 (Fiori & Longo, 2018;Longo, Ghosh, & Yahya, 2015). Intriguingly, such illusions mirror 36 anisotropies in the geometry of tactile RFs which in animals tend to be oval-shaped both in 37 the spinal cord (e.g., Brown, Fuchs, & Tapper, 1975) and in SI (e.g., Alloway, Rosenthal, & 38 Burton, 1989;Brooks, Rudomin, & Slayman, 1961) with the longer axis aligned with the 39 proximo-distal body axis.…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 74%
“…This overall pattern has been replicated numerous times in several labs (Cocchini et al in press;Coelho et al 2017;Coelho and Gonzalez in press;Ferrè et al 2012;Ganea and Longo 2017;Longo 2014Longo , 2015Longo , 2017bHaggard 2012a, 2012b;Longo et Response Type and Perceptual Hand Maps 4 al. 2012, 2015a, 2015bLongo and Morcom 2016;Lopez et al 2012;Mattioni and Longo 2014;Medina and Duckett 2017;Saulton et al 2015Saulton et al , 2016Saulton et al , 2017Stone et al 2018;Tamè et al 2017). Nevertheless, the interpretation of such effects remains uncertain.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%