2011
DOI: 10.1016/j.visres.2010.09.032
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The influence of orientation and contrast flicker on contour saliency of outlines of everyday objects

Abstract: One of the most important tasks of the visual system is the extraction of edges and object contours, and the integration of discrete elements to form a coherent global percept. A great deal is known about the spatial properties of contour extraction, but less is known about the dynamics and spatio-temporal aspects. We used Gabor-rendered outlines of real-world objects, where we could manipulate low-level properties, such as element orientation and phase, while incorporating higher-level properties, such as obj… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

1
13
0

Year Published

2011
2011
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

4
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 11 publications
(14 citation statements)
references
References 46 publications
(67 reference statements)
1
13
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In one of these studies, we have tested detection of shapes as a function of contour and surface grouping (Machilsen and Wagemans 2011), whereas most of the others have added shape identity as well by implementing these grouping cues in recognizable outlines, creating so-called Gaborized outlines (Nygård et al 2009, 2011; Sassi et al 2010). …”
Section: Parallellepipeda: From Parallel Worlds To Interacting Peoplementioning
confidence: 99%
“…In one of these studies, we have tested detection of shapes as a function of contour and surface grouping (Machilsen and Wagemans 2011), whereas most of the others have added shape identity as well by implementing these grouping cues in recognizable outlines, creating so-called Gaborized outlines (Nygård et al 2009, 2011; Sassi et al 2010). …”
Section: Parallellepipeda: From Parallel Worlds To Interacting Peoplementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Machilsen and Wagemans (2010) have previously shown that an interior texture consisting of a cluster of similarly oriented elements embedded in a background of randomly oriented elements allows shape detection even in the absence of curvilinear contour elements, whereas earlier work by Nygård (2009) showed that such textures without contours do not allow shape identification (see also Nygård, 2009; Nygård, Sassi, & Wagemans, 2011). For this reason, we expected the OCP version, of which the interior and exterior could be segmented on the basis of the difference in average orientation, to show a clear advantage over the other versions in the detection task, where contour integration need not be the limiting factor.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Traditionally, the most common approach is to place background elements according to an underlying grid and then add positional noise (e.g., Altmann, Deubelius, & Kourtzi, 2004;Bex, Simmers, & Dakin, 2001;Dumoulin & Hess, 2006;Field et al, 1993;Kourtzi, Tolias, Altmann, Augath, & Logothetis, 2003;Kuai & Yu, 2006;Li & Gilbert, 2002;Mathes & Fahle, 2007a, b;Mathes, Trenner, & Fahle, 2006;May & Hess, 2007;Mullen, Beaudot, & McIlhagga, 2000;Nygård, Sassi, & Wagemans, 2011;Tanskanen, Saarinen, Parkkonen, & Hari, 2008). In a similar fashion, Hadad, Maurer, and Lewis (2010) placed background elements on imaginary concentric circles around the embedded closed contour.…”
Section: Background Elementsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…An unwanted difference in spacing between contour elements and background elements may then invalidate the conclusions. Many authors (e.g., Braun, 1999;Geisler et al, 2001;Li & Gilbert, 2002;Mathes & Fahle, 2007b;Mullen et al, 2000;Nygård et al, 2009Nygård et al, , 2011Tversky et al, 2004) and even more peer referees warn against local density cues.…”
Section: Background Elementsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation