1980
DOI: 10.3758/bf03198826
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The influence of imagery ability on color aftereffects produced by physically present and imagined induction stimuli

Abstract: Two methods of induction were used to produce orientation-contingent color aftereffects for observers assigned to one of three groups (high, medium, and low) on the basis of self-rated imagery ability. In Experiment 1, observers were required to make magnitude estimates of color aftereffects following inspection of stimulus patterns normally used to produce McCollough effects (e.g., red vertical contours, green horizontal contours). Experiment 2 was a partial replication of Experiment 1, with additional induct… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1

Citation Types

1
33
0

Year Published

1981
1981
2013
2013

Publication Types

Select...
9

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 26 publications
(34 citation statements)
references
References 33 publications
1
33
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The difficulty of replicating many of the more impressive findings of image-percept equivalence (Broerse & Crassini, 1980, 1981Intons-Peterson & White, 1981) has led some psychologists to consider the role of experimenter expectancy effects in imagery research. For example, Intons-Peterson (1983) has contended that the experimental paradigms used to study the visual properties of imagery are sufficiently vulnerable to experimenter expectancy that much, if not all, of the data showing visual properties of images could be artifactual.…”
Section: Alternative Explanations Of Findings In Cognitive Psychologymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The difficulty of replicating many of the more impressive findings of image-percept equivalence (Broerse & Crassini, 1980, 1981Intons-Peterson & White, 1981) has led some psychologists to consider the role of experimenter expectancy effects in imagery research. For example, Intons-Peterson (1983) has contended that the experimental paradigms used to study the visual properties of imagery are sufficiently vulnerable to experimenter expectancy that much, if not all, of the data showing visual properties of images could be artifactual.…”
Section: Alternative Explanations Of Findings In Cognitive Psychologymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Hoen (1978) found a low, but significant, positive correlation between scores on the VVIQ and Form C of the Stanford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale. On this view, one would expect an "experimenter effect" in studies of lIMEs, which may be one of the reasons why Broerse and Crassini (1980) failed to find them.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In view of the recent discussion (Broerse & Crassini, 1980Finke, 1981) of the relationship between self-ratings of vividness of visual imagery and imagery-induced McCollough effects (Finke & Schmidt, 1977, 1978Kunen & May, 1980;Kaufman, May, & Kunen, 1981), it seemed worthwhile to look again at the original data (Finke & Schmidt, 1978, Table I) and examine how unequivocally they support the conclusion that subjects with visual imagery self-rated as more vivid report stronger imageryinduced McCollough effects (lIMEs). The reanalysis suggests an alternative view of the data, and of other effects which follow adaptation to partially imaginary stimuli.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…I would like to comment on two articles, recently published in this journal (Broerse & Crassini, 1980;Kunen & May, 1980),which explored the finding that mental images can be used to establish the McCollough effect, reported by M. J. Schmidt and myself several years ago (Finke & Schmidt, 1977, 1978. In particular, I would like to examine the evidence provided in these recent studies pertaining to the proposal that imagined and observed patterns are functionally equivalent.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%