2003
DOI: 10.1080/0950069022000038303
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The impact of the MARS curriculum on students' ability to coordinate theory and evidence

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
18
0
2

Year Published

2004
2004
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 31 publications
(21 citation statements)
references
References 29 publications
1
18
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Students from the MARS curriculum have also been shown to perform better on hands-on and paper-andpencil inquiry tasks when compared to students instructed with a nationally-recognized NSF-supported inquiry curriculum (Zimmerman et al 2003). The MARS curriculum had been implemented in many schools throughout the region with various content modules for each middle school grade.…”
Section: Participantsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Students from the MARS curriculum have also been shown to perform better on hands-on and paper-andpencil inquiry tasks when compared to students instructed with a nationally-recognized NSF-supported inquiry curriculum (Zimmerman et al 2003). The MARS curriculum had been implemented in many schools throughout the region with various content modules for each middle school grade.…”
Section: Participantsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One of its primary goals is to enhance science-reasoning skills among middle school students of diverse backgrounds. The curriculum focuses on helping middle school students reason about science ideas through the generation, use, and refinement of models (Zimmerman et al 2003). A central aspect of the instruction in MARS is a computer software environment that allows students to generate a theoretical model of a situation, run their generated model in the software to view the outcome that their model predicts, and then compare that prediction to a run of the ''real'' model (i.e., the canonical science model).…”
Section: Participantsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…author, 2007;Chinn & Brewer;1998;Hainsworth, 1956Hainsworth, & 1958McCormas & Moore, 2001;Rigano & Richie, 1995;Watson, Swain & McRobbie;2004;Zimmerman et al, 2003). The authors of one study of experimenter expectancy in school biology pupils (aged 14-19) describe the effect potentially being a universal problem that can apply to almost any kind of scientific measurement in the classroom (McCormas & Moore, 2001).…”
Section: Theoretical Backgroundmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Even during enquiry experiments, when faced with a theory/evidence disparity they tend to assume an overly theory-laden mind set and automatically reject their empirical results out of hand (e.g. author, 2010;Austin, Holding, Bell & Daniels, 1991;Gunstone & Champagne, 1990;Lubben & Millar, 1996;Zimmerman, Raghavan & Sartoris, 2003). Pupils may have difficulties switching between the positivistic epistemology of the more common illustrative practicals and the constructivist epistemology of the usually rarer hypothetico-deductive open ended enquiry tasks (author, 2011).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Research with older children, however, has revealed that 8-to 12-year-olds have limitations in their abilities to (a) generate unconfounded experiments, (b) disconfirm hypotheses, (c) keep accurate and systematic records, and (d) evaluate evidence (Klahr, Fay, & Dunbar, 1993;Kuhn, Garcia-Mila, Zohar, & Andersen, 1995;Schauble, 1990Schauble, , 1996Zimmerman, Raghavan, & Sartoris, 2003). For example, Schauble (1990) presented children aged 9-11 with a computerized task in which they had to determine which of five factors affect the speed of racing cars.…”
Section: Metacognitive and Metastrategic Processesmentioning
confidence: 99%