2013
DOI: 10.1080/02687038.2013.770816
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The impact of impaired semantic knowledge on spontaneous iconic gesture production

Abstract: BackgroundPrevious research has found that people with aphasia produce more spontaneous iconic gesture than control participants, especially during word-finding difficulties. There is some evidence that impaired semantic knowledge impacts on the diversity of gestural handshapes, as well as the frequency of gesture production. However, no previous research has explored how impaired semantic knowledge impacts on the frequency and type of iconic gestures produced during fluent speech compared with those produced … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

4
54
2
1

Year Published

2017
2017
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
1

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 36 publications
(61 citation statements)
references
References 21 publications
4
54
2
1
Order By: Relevance
“…It is more important to note that the fact that our nonfluent PWA had a tendency to produce fewer words and complete sentences than their counterparts with fluent aphasia and that they were more impaired in oral naming abilities than fluent PWA echoed this claim by de Ruiter and de Beer. However, we acknowledge that this finding regarding a different gesture rate by fluency groups of PWA could have been confounded by aphasia severity because the fluent PWA in the present study were generally Cocks et al (2013), in which information in speakers' gestures produced during fluent speech as well as during word-finding difficulties were linked to a speaker's semantic knowledge.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 77%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…It is more important to note that the fact that our nonfluent PWA had a tendency to produce fewer words and complete sentences than their counterparts with fluent aphasia and that they were more impaired in oral naming abilities than fluent PWA echoed this claim by de Ruiter and de Beer. However, we acknowledge that this finding regarding a different gesture rate by fluency groups of PWA could have been confounded by aphasia severity because the fluent PWA in the present study were generally Cocks et al (2013), in which information in speakers' gestures produced during fluent speech as well as during word-finding difficulties were linked to a speaker's semantic knowledge.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 77%
“…This finding suggested concomitant impairment between gestural and linguistic skills in PWA. In contrast, it has also been reported that a significantly greater proportion of PWA used gestures, including iconic gestures (Cocks, Dipper, Pritchard, & Morgan, 2013), than their unimpaired counterparts, and many of the PWA produced iconic gestures that served communicative and/or facilitative functions . This observation supported the sketch model (de Ruiter, 2000) and provided evidence for independent deficits of gesture and verbal production.…”
Section: Aphasia and Gesture Usementioning
confidence: 89%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The study of individuals with aphasia forms an important part of our understanding of the relationship between gesture and speech (e g Ahlsén, 1991; Cicone et al, 1979; Cocks, Dipper, Pritchard, & Morgan, 2013; Dipper, Cocks, Rowe, & Morgan, 2011; Hadar et al, 1998; Pritchard, Cocks, & Dipper, 2013; Wilkinson, 2013). If co-speech gestures and speech are produced by a common system of processing, then one would expect impairments in one system to be reflected in the other (see Eling & Whitaker, 2009; McNeill, 1985, 1992; McNeill et al, 2008).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As to the possibility of compensation, there is evidence that while individuals with severe deficits in nonverbal semantic processing (Hogrefe et al, 2012) and semantic knowledge (Cocks et al, 2013) cannot use nonverbal tools efficiently for interpersonal communication, patients with intact semantic processing can use gesture as a compensatory strategy for communication (Göksun et al, 2015; Hogrefe et al, 2012). Thus, we expected that people with damage to language-related areas of the brain (the left temporal lobe and left inferior frontal cortex) to produce more gestures to compensate for their verbal impairment, provided that they do not have semantic deficiencies.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%