2012
DOI: 10.1027/1614-2241/a000048
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Impact of Controlling for Extreme Responding on Measurement Equivalence in Cross-Cultural Research

Abstract: Prior research has shown that extreme response style can seriously bias responses to survey questions and that this response style may differ across culturally diverse groups. Consequently, cross-cultural differences in extreme responding may yield incomparable responses when not controlled for. To examine how extreme responding affects the cross-cultural comparability of survey responses, we propose and apply a multiple-group latent class approach where groups are compared on basis of the factor loadings, int… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

1
31
0

Year Published

2013
2013
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
4
3

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 37 publications
(34 citation statements)
references
References 42 publications
1
31
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The threshold parameters were set to −0.6, 0, and 0.6 for the 4-category items and −0.8, −0.4, 0, 0.4, and 0.8 for the 6-category items. The specifications of the model parameters were consistent with those that are commonly found in practice and similar to previous research (e.g., Li et al, 2006; Morren et al, 2012; Jin and Wang, 2014a; Huang, 2014, 2015, 2016). Each condition was replicated 30 times.…”
Section: Methodssupporting
confidence: 88%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…The threshold parameters were set to −0.6, 0, and 0.6 for the 4-category items and −0.8, −0.4, 0, 0.4, and 0.8 for the 6-category items. The specifications of the model parameters were consistent with those that are commonly found in practice and similar to previous research (e.g., Li et al, 2006; Morren et al, 2012; Jin and Wang, 2014a; Huang, 2014, 2015, 2016). Each condition was replicated 30 times.…”
Section: Methodssupporting
confidence: 88%
“…In turn, these problems can confound the nuisance ERS effect and the measure-intended dimension (Cheung and Rensvold, 2000; Bolt and Johnson, 2009; Thissen-Roe and Thissen, 2013). In fact, if controls for ERS are absent, inferences regarding cross-cultural comparisons and measurement equivalence are misleading (Morren et al, 2012). Under real testing situations, differences in ERS tendencies among respondents and differences in item parameters among response styles may occur simultaneously; thus, their effects should be distinguished in a latent trait model.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…Some authors attempted to control for response styles by either studying ipsative measures or standardized individual scores (e.g., M. W.-L. Cheung & Chan, 2002;Fischer, 2004) or by conducting analyses of partial correlations (e.g., ten Berge, 1999). Moreover, researchers made use of structural equation models (e.g., Billiet & McClendon, 2000), item response models (e.g., De Jong et al, 2008;Jin & Wang, 2013;Johnson & Bolt, 2010), latent class or mixed Rasch models (e.g., Gollwitzer, Eid, & Jürgensen, 2005;Meiser & Machunsky, 2008;Morren, Gelissen, & Vermunt, 2012), and other, more specialized approaches (e.g., Camparo & Camparo, 2013;Johnson, 2003;Rossi, Gilula, & Allenby, 2001). Finally, Weijters and colleagues employed sophisticated factorial models in the framework of structural equation modeling (e.g., Weijters, Geuens, et al, 2010a).…”
Section: Research Methods To Investigate Response Stylesmentioning
confidence: 99%