2016
DOI: 10.1016/j.jcps.2016.03.002
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The ‘I’ in extreme responding

Abstract: We investigate the impact of self‐construal on extreme responding in six studies. The results show that people with an independent self‐construal generally answer more extremely to survey items than those with an interdependent self‐construal, especially when the items are self‐relevant (Studies 1a and 1b) and when these items are fluently processed (Study 3). Using an experimental causal chain design, this research also demonstrates that self‐concept clarity drives the effect of self‐construal on extreme resp… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
14
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 12 publications
(15 citation statements)
references
References 79 publications
(141 reference statements)
1
14
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In this regard, researchers usually distinguish between perceptual and conceptual fluency. Perceptual fluency refers to the ease of identifying the perceptual features of a stimulus (e.g., a product design or an advertisement), such as its form, size, or visual details (Reber, Schwarz, & Winkielman, 2004;Schwarz, 2004), and has been shown to be influenced by variables such as repeated exposure (Bornstein & D'Agostino, 1994;Fang, Singh, & Ahluwalia, 2007;Landwehr et al, 2017), the font in which information is presented (Cabooter, Millet, Weijters, & Pandelaere, 2016;DeMotta, Chao, & Kramer, 2016), visual clarity (Whittlesea, Jacoby, & Girard, 1990), complexity (Landwehr et al, 2011), or figureground contrast (Reber, Winkielman, & Schwarz, 1998;Thompson & Ince, 2013). Conceptual fluency, by contrast, refers to the ease of mental operations concerned with assigning meaning to a stimulus (e.g., to a product; Lee & Labroo, 2004;Reber, Schwarz, & Winkielman, 2004;Reber, Wurtz, & Zimmermann, 2004), and empirical evidence suggests that this fluency can arise when a product is presented in a predictive context or when it is primed by a related construct (Lee & Labroo, 2004).…”
Section: Theoretical Definition and Foundationmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In this regard, researchers usually distinguish between perceptual and conceptual fluency. Perceptual fluency refers to the ease of identifying the perceptual features of a stimulus (e.g., a product design or an advertisement), such as its form, size, or visual details (Reber, Schwarz, & Winkielman, 2004;Schwarz, 2004), and has been shown to be influenced by variables such as repeated exposure (Bornstein & D'Agostino, 1994;Fang, Singh, & Ahluwalia, 2007;Landwehr et al, 2017), the font in which information is presented (Cabooter, Millet, Weijters, & Pandelaere, 2016;DeMotta, Chao, & Kramer, 2016), visual clarity (Whittlesea, Jacoby, & Girard, 1990), complexity (Landwehr et al, 2011), or figureground contrast (Reber, Winkielman, & Schwarz, 1998;Thompson & Ince, 2013). Conceptual fluency, by contrast, refers to the ease of mental operations concerned with assigning meaning to a stimulus (e.g., to a product; Lee & Labroo, 2004;Reber, Schwarz, & Winkielman, 2004;Reber, Wurtz, & Zimmermann, 2004), and empirical evidence suggests that this fluency can arise when a product is presented in a predictive context or when it is primed by a related construct (Lee & Labroo, 2004).…”
Section: Theoretical Definition and Foundationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…That is, of the 120 articles that used a direct measure of the subjective fluency experience, roughly one half (i.e., 45%) used multiple items to measure fluency, whereas the other half (i.e., 51.67%) used only a single item to measure fluency (3.33% used both a multi-item measure and a single-item measure). Notably, most singleitem measures employ some variation of the items easy and/or difficult (e.g., Cabooter et al, 2016;Fang et al, 2007;Lembregts & Pandelaere, 2012;Shen, Jiang, & Adaval, 2010). In fact, the two items difficult and easy, which are also often used within the reviewed multi-item measures (e.g., DeMotta et al, 2016;Labroo & Lee, 2006;Landwehr et al, 2011;Lee, Keller, & Sternthal, 2010), closely reflect the theoretical definition of processing fluency.…”
Section: Measures Of the Subjective Fluency Experiencementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Third, response scale usage can vary as a function of often manipulated variables such as self-regulatory focus or self-construal (Cabooter, Millet, Weijters, & Pandelaere, 2016;Lalwani, Shrum, & Chiu, 2009). Other experimental manipulations may also lead to a possibly unintended shift in response category usage (e.g., differences in ambient lighting, questionnaire readability, etc.).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Research has shown that shorter latency when providing self-descriptive traits is an indicator of higher levels of SCC (Cabooter, Millet, et al, 2016;Campbell et al, 1996). This type of measure for SCC also eliminates biases (e.g., social desirability, reactivity) in self-reported measures Campbell, 1990).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 95%
“…First, they completed the SCC measure. Participants responded to a version of the Twenty-Statement Task, adapted from Cabooter, Millet, Weijters, and Pandelaere (2016), as a measure of SCC. Participants provided as many self-descriptive traits as possible (with a maximum of 10), while we measured the time it took them to complete this task in seconds.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%