Our system is currently under heavy load due to increased usage. We're actively working on upgrades to improve performance. Thank you for your patience.
2011
DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.03.071
|View full text |Cite|
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The genetics of colored sequence synesthesia: Suggestive evidence of linkage to 16q and genetic heterogeneity for the condition

Abstract: Synesthesia is a perceptual condition in which sensory stimulation triggers anomalous sensory experiences. In colored sequence synesthesia (CSS), color experiences are triggered by sequences such as letters or numbers. We performed a family based linkage analysis to identify genetic loci responsible for the increased neural crosstalk underlying CSS. Our results implicate a 23 MB region at 16q12.2-23.1, providing the first step in understanding the molecular basis of CSS.

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

5
62
0
3

Year Published

2012
2012
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
5
4

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 81 publications
(70 citation statements)
references
References 45 publications
5
62
0
3
Order By: Relevance
“…grapheme and colour, or sound and shape) are found in only about 2-4% of the population (Simner et al, 2006;Ward, 2013). The low prevalence of cases is consistent with suggestions that synaesthesia represents an aberrant genotype (Brang, Williams, & Ramachandran, 2012;Tomson et al, 2011), which may result in unusual patterns of neural crosswiring or cross-activation between adjacent cortical regions (Ramachandran & Hubbard, 2001;Rouw & Scholte, 2007). However, many researchers have argued that some forms of synaesthesia might be grounded on normal mechanisms involved in forming and reinforcing associations between different modalities and sensory dimensions (Brang, Williams, & Ramachandran, 2012;Cohen Kadosh, Henik, Catena, Walsh, & Fuentes, 2009;Cytowic, 2003;Grossenbacher & Lovelace, 2001; Ramachandran & Hubbard, 2001;Ward, Huckstep, & Tsakanikos, 2006).…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 82%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…grapheme and colour, or sound and shape) are found in only about 2-4% of the population (Simner et al, 2006;Ward, 2013). The low prevalence of cases is consistent with suggestions that synaesthesia represents an aberrant genotype (Brang, Williams, & Ramachandran, 2012;Tomson et al, 2011), which may result in unusual patterns of neural crosswiring or cross-activation between adjacent cortical regions (Ramachandran & Hubbard, 2001;Rouw & Scholte, 2007). However, many researchers have argued that some forms of synaesthesia might be grounded on normal mechanisms involved in forming and reinforcing associations between different modalities and sensory dimensions (Brang, Williams, & Ramachandran, 2012;Cohen Kadosh, Henik, Catena, Walsh, & Fuentes, 2009;Cytowic, 2003;Grossenbacher & Lovelace, 2001; Ramachandran & Hubbard, 2001;Ward, Huckstep, & Tsakanikos, 2006).…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 82%
“…In terms of brain mechanisms, it is currently debated to what extent synaesthesias reflect genotypically unusual cross-wiring between sensory areas (Brang et al, 2012;Tomson et al, 2011), versus physiological disinhibition of normally-occurring connections (Cohen Kadosh et al, 2009;Cohen Kadosh & Walsh, 2006;Grossenbacher & Lovelace, 2001;Neufeld et al, 2012). This debate might be informed by whether the behavioural measures have a discontinuous or continuous distribution (Cohen Kadosh, 2013;Deroy & Spence, 2015;Martino & Marks, 2001;Simner, 2012).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the vernacular of synaesthesia research, an "inducer" (e.g., letter) triggers a synaesthetic "concurrent" (e.g., colour). The condition has a genetic basis (Asher et al, 2009;Tomson et al, 2011) and gives rise to anatomical differences including altered white-matter coherence (e.g., Rouw & Scholte, 2007) and grey matter volume (Weiss & Fink, 2009). Although synaesthetes' experiences are superficially idiosyncratic (e.g., the letter A might be red for one synaesthete but blue for another), underlying patterns exist in the associations between the inducer and concurrent, and these patterns are often also found in nonsynaesthetes' intuitive cross-modal associations.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In these terms, OLP could be categorised as a form of developmental synaesthesia. Synaesthesia is a genetically linked condition (Asher et al, 2009;Tomson et al, 2011) in which every-day stimuli trigger unusual secondary asso iatio s. A het pal a ifestatio s of s aesthesia i lude ases he e t o se ses a e e ged in some way, such as when music triggers visual perceptions of colour (e.g., Ward, Huckstep & Tsakanikos, 2006) or where tastes trigger tactile sensations of shape (e.g., Cytowic, 1993). However, the neural conditions giving rise to synaesthesia (e.g., atypical white-matter coherence; see below)…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%