1988
DOI: 10.1037/0278-7393.14.1.180
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The generation effect with nonwords.

Abstract: This article challenges the view that the generation effects occurs only for items represented in semantic memory. We obtained generation effects regardless of the lexical status of the cue used to generate the target and regardless of the lexical status of the target. Generation effects were comparable for wordlike and unwordlike nonwords. The effect for nonwords depended on displaying the target at the end of each generate trial. We argue that such feedback gives subjects appropriate visual experience with n… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
50
0

Year Published

1988
1988
2008
2008

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 53 publications
(54 citation statements)
references
References 20 publications
4
50
0
Order By: Relevance
“…A simple explanation is that subjects have some memory for the literal form of generated stimuli (cf. Johns & Swanson, 1988;Nairne & Widner, 1987). That little extra memory aids discrimination between semantically similar but orthographically distinct alternatives, and hurts discrimination between orthographically similar but semantically distinct alternatives.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…A simple explanation is that subjects have some memory for the literal form of generated stimuli (cf. Johns & Swanson, 1988;Nairne & Widner, 1987). That little extra memory aids discrimination between semantically similar but orthographically distinct alternatives, and hurts discrimination between orthographically similar but semantically distinct alternatives.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, if generators see ohrven and transpose letters mentally to produce "horven" as a response, whereas readers see ohrven and horven side by side, then a test for horven involves a stimulus that the readers have actually seen, but that thegenerators have not. Johns and Swanson (1988) found that testing with the complete forms decreased recognition for generated nonwords; there was a generation effect when the fragments were the tested stimuli, or when the complete form had been given as feedback after the generative attempt. To avoid a study-test confound, we can test with the studied fragments, or we can provide feedback after the attempt, to define the unit that the experimenter has in mind.…”
Section: Generated Targets Benefit From Generationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…That is, when subjects generate nonsense responses (it was expected that) there should be no generation effect for the responses and, therefore, the generation effect for cues could be sought on multiple memory measures. Subsequent to conducting Experiment 3, however, demonstrations of generation effects for nonsense syllables were reported (Johns & Swanson, 1988;Nairne & Widner, 1987), retrospectively undermining a component of the plan of Experiment 3. 4 As will be seen, however, Experiment 3 was nevertheless able to provide useful evidence bearing on the generation effect for cues.…”
Section: Experiments 3 a Further Test Of A Generation Effect For Cuesmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In experiments in which college students generated (as compared to read) free associates or rhymes, memory was enhanced (e.g., Schwartz & Metcalfe, 1992;Slamecka & Graf, 1978). The effect has been shown to occur with words (Jacoby, 1978), sentences (Graf, 1980), bigrams (Gardiner & Hampton, 1985), numbers (Gardiner & Rowley, 1984), and pictures (Peynircioglu, 1989), so long as the format of the test is the same as the format of study (Johns & Swanson, 1988;Nairne & Widner, 1987). In most studies, the items have been carefully selected such that in the generate condition, the participant always generated the correct answer.…”
Section: Self-generation Of Responsesmentioning
confidence: 99%