2008
DOI: 10.3758/pp.70.2.329
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The encoding process of nonconfigural information in contextual cuing

Abstract: Jiang and Wagner (2004) demonstrated that individual target-distractor associations were learned in contextual cuing. We examined whether individual associations can be learned in efficient visual searches that do not involve attentional deployment to individual search items. In Experiment 1, individual associations were not learned during the efficient search tasks. However, in Experiment 2, where additional exposure duration of the search display was provided by presenting placeholders marking future locatio… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

4
14
2

Year Published

2010
2010
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 13 publications
(20 citation statements)
references
References 11 publications
4
14
2
Order By: Relevance
“…They are however inconsistent with the conclusions of other research (Jiang & Wagner, 2004, Experiment 1; see also Jiang & Song, 2005;Ogawa & Kumada, 2008) in which search was unimpaired in a transfer phase in which trained patterns were recombined into new configurations. We return to this discrepancy later in the General Discussion.…”
Section: Discussioncontrasting
confidence: 95%
“…They are however inconsistent with the conclusions of other research (Jiang & Wagner, 2004, Experiment 1; see also Jiang & Song, 2005;Ogawa & Kumada, 2008) in which search was unimpaired in a transfer phase in which trained patterns were recombined into new configurations. We return to this discrepancy later in the General Discussion.…”
Section: Discussioncontrasting
confidence: 95%
“…Peterson and Kramer (2001) discussed the possibility that contextual cueing may have started while the placeholders were in view, although this could not be determined from their data. Geyer et al (2010) and Ogawa and Kumada (2008) used placeholders to enhance contextual cueing. In both studies, however, the search task involved feature search, which may rely primarily on the global layout for cueing.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Unfortunately, none of the previous behavioral studies directly measured the time course of contextual cueing. The interval between the onset of the premask placeholders and the search items was constant in most cases (Geyer, Zehetleitner, & Müller, 2010;Ogawa & Kumada, 2008;Peterson & Kramer, 2001). Kunar et al (2008) found that previews ranging from 400 to 1,200 ms improved search speeds.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…For instance, it is possible that in changed displays, the target and the distractors may be grouped together, thus forming a segmentable region (Conci & von Mühlenen, 2009) or a 'salient' cluster of items that is prioritized for the assignment of bottom-up attention. Thus, because the search display is removed upon the response, insufficient time may be available for the system to encode, i.e., (re-)learn, the spatial targetdistractor relations (e.g., Ogawa & Kumada, 2008) or there may be no incentive for (re-) learning as the target is found efficiently. Note that these accounts are neutral as to whether bottom-up attention affects the processes of the build-up, i.e., acquisition of a new association between the changed target position and the old distractor context or the expression, i.e., retrieval of this (adapted) representation from context memory.…”
Section: Lack-of-adaption Of Contextual Cueingmentioning
confidence: 99%