2012
DOI: 10.3758/s13421-012-0247-8
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The emotion-induced memory trade-off: More than an effect of overt attention?

Abstract: Although it has been suggested that many effects of emotion on memory are attributable to attention, in the present study we addressed the hypothesis that such effects may relate to a number of different factors during encoding or postencoding. One way to look at the effects of emotion on memory is by examining the emotion-induced memory trade-off, whereby enhanced memory for emotional items often comes at the cost of memory for surrounding background information. We present evidence that this trade-off cannot… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

5
56
0

Year Published

2013
2013
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
3
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 50 publications
(61 citation statements)
references
References 41 publications
5
56
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The time for completion and eye search pattern data were not affected by the emotional stimulus conditions. The recall test results corroborate the study by Steimetz and Kensinger [26] which shows that selective memory for emotional information is not strongly related to attention at encoding. In fact, although we have observed a positive effect in the task performance itself with neutral and positive images, this finding was not replicated in the recall, where we did not find significant differences between positive, negative and neutral stimuli.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 88%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The time for completion and eye search pattern data were not affected by the emotional stimulus conditions. The recall test results corroborate the study by Steimetz and Kensinger [26] which shows that selective memory for emotional information is not strongly related to attention at encoding. In fact, although we have observed a positive effect in the task performance itself with neutral and positive images, this finding was not replicated in the recall, where we did not find significant differences between positive, negative and neutral stimuli.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 88%
“…An example is the weapon focus effect, where there is enhanced memory for a weapon in a scene but reduced memory for details of the background [25]. This focus may lead to selective memory for emotional components [24], although some researchers argue that selective memory of emotional content is not strongly related to attention at encoding [26].…”
Section: The Role Of Emotional Stimuli In Rehabilitationmentioning
confidence: 97%
“…According to this theory, "emotional arousal increases the competitive advantage of stimulus priority, leading to 'winner-take-more' and 'loser-take-less' effects in the competition for limited resources" (Sutherland & Mather, 2012, p. 1367. Steinmetz and Kensinger (2013) proposed a similar "emotion-induced memory trade-off, whereby enhanced memory for emotional items often comes at the cost of memory for surrounding information" (p. 69). Further, the extent of the trade-off between good memory for emotional items and poor memory for surrounding items may depend on task demands.…”
mentioning
confidence: 96%
“…When a stimulus is inherently emotionally arousing, it has a competitive advantage over stimuli nearby in space or time (Bradley et al, 2012). For instance, people tend to remember foreground objects better when they are emotional than neutral, but at the cost of poorer memory for the background scenes (Steinmetz and Kensinger, 2013). These effects favoring emotionally salient objects over the surrounding less salient context become stronger with a night of sleep (Payne et al, 2012).…”
Section: Emotion and Selectivity In Attention And Memorymentioning
confidence: 99%