1966
DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1966.tb01697.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The effects of type and strength of induced conflict on conflict generalization and later preference for conflict stimuli1

Abstract: Recently, research has been undertaken m the area of conflict which attempts to deal with the problem of the generalization of conflict effects (Worell, 1962a, Worell, 1962b, Worell, 1963, WoreU & Castaneda, 1961 The aim has been to determme whether prior exposure to different mtensities of conflict affects performance m subsequent conflict situations To date, the results mdicate that the more severe the initial conflict trammg, the greater is the impairment of decision efficiency m a subsequent performance s… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1

Citation Types

2
11
0

Year Published

1975
1975
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(13 citation statements)
references
References 21 publications
2
11
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In a series of studies that followed Barker's lead, several investigators (e.g., Arkoff, 1957; Minor, Miller, & Ditrichs, 1968; Murray, 1975; Schill, 1966) demonstrated that making choices between obtaining mutually exclusive unfavorable personality characteristics took significantly longer and was judged as significantly more difficult than making choices between obtaining one of two favorable personality characteristics. Moreover, when an undecided response category was introduced into the task (Murray, 1975), subjects were much more likely to use it in negative avoidance–avoidance conflicts (42%) than in positive approach–approach conflicts (6%).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In a series of studies that followed Barker's lead, several investigators (e.g., Arkoff, 1957; Minor, Miller, & Ditrichs, 1968; Murray, 1975; Schill, 1966) demonstrated that making choices between obtaining mutually exclusive unfavorable personality characteristics took significantly longer and was judged as significantly more difficult than making choices between obtaining one of two favorable personality characteristics. Moreover, when an undecided response category was introduced into the task (Murray, 1975), subjects were much more likely to use it in negative avoidance–avoidance conflicts (42%) than in positive approach–approach conflicts (6%).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…He found that participants took longer to resolve the AV-AV conflicts and judged them to be more difficult to resolve, compared to the AP-AP conflicts. Subsequent research has produced similar results for both decision time (Atthowe, 1960;Barker, 1942;Boyd et al, 2011;Epstein & Smith, 1967;Heitmann & Deutsch, 2018;Houston et al, 1991;Minor et al, 1968;Murray, 1975;Ringuette, 1965;Schill, 1966;Terry, 2010) and experienced difficulty (Chatterjee & Heath, 1996;Minor et al, 1968;Murray, 1975;Terry, 2010). There is thus a widespread agreement, based on sound theoretical logic, and robust and replicable experimental evidence, that AV-AV conflicts are more difficult to resolve than AP-AP ones.…”
mentioning
confidence: 78%
“…E. Miller’s (1944) theory, based on the notion of the dynamic operation of force fields, suggested that the two types of motivational conflicts differ in the relative difficulty of resolving them, such that AV–AV conflicts are more difficult to resolve than AP–AP conflicts. Subsequent research has almost exclusively focused on this main effect of difference in conflict resolution difficulty (e.g., Arkoff, 1957; Barker, 1946; Barker, 1942; Boyd et al, 2011; Heitmann & Deutsch, 2018; Houston et al, 1991; Minor et al, 1968; Murray, 1975; Ringuette, 1965; Schill, 1966; Terry, 2010). However, for the most part, it did not consider, nor explore, contexts that can moderate this difference in the relative difficulty of resolving AP–AP and AV–AV conflicts (see a discussion of a few notable exceptions below).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…One exception to this is a study reported by Schill (1966) in which conflicts were devised based on the subjects' own ratings of adjectives. In light of Ringuette's (1976) findings, it is reasonable to assume that individuals define conflict in certain individualistic ways, as well as common ways, and that their responses to such self-defined conflicts will differ from their responses to other-defined conflicts.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%