1975
DOI: 10.3758/bf03213426
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The effects of strong irrelevant thirst on food-rewarded instrumental performance

Abstract: .In Experiment I, rats received one food rewarded trial per day in a runway. One group received all its trials under hunger (Group H); the second group received a random half of its trials under hunger and the other half of its trials under hunger plus thirst (Group H·HT). Group H·HT ultimately ran slower on HT trials than on H trials. In Experiment II, the effects of shifting from H to-HT-and vice versa were examined in a five-phase design. In general, rats run under H ran faster than rats run under HT, and s… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
8
0

Year Published

1987
1987
2014
2014

Publication Types

Select...
5

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 17 publications
0
8
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The strength of this association is said to govern the vigor of the CR and to be determined in part by the magnitude of the US. Because thirst may reduce the palatability of food (Bolles, 1975;Capaldi et al, 1975), it is possible that the presence of this irrelevant drive will reduce the effectiveness of food as a reinforcer for Pavlovian conditioning. This will result in the growth of a weaker CS-US association in animals with the irrelevant drive than in animals that are only hungry.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The strength of this association is said to govern the vigor of the CR and to be determined in part by the magnitude of the US. Because thirst may reduce the palatability of food (Bolles, 1975;Capaldi et al, 1975), it is possible that the presence of this irrelevant drive will reduce the effectiveness of food as a reinforcer for Pavlovian conditioning. This will result in the growth of a weaker CS-US association in animals with the irrelevant drive than in animals that are only hungry.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Kendler (1945) found a clear facilitation of extinction in the group with the most severe water deprivation (22 hr) relative to a group that was merely food deprived. Since then, it has been repeatedly found in acquisition that high levels of irrelevant thirst impair instrumental responding for food (Capaldi, Hovancik, & Lamb, 1975) and extinction (e.g., Siegel, 1946) and also that irrelevant hunger is similarly detrimental to water-reinforced responding (e.g., Bolles & Morlock, 1960;Kendler & Law, 1950).Parallel to these instrumental conditioning studies, other authors have noted decreased feeding when water deprivation is imposed on the animal and decreased drinking when food deprivation is in progress (e.g., Toates, 1980; Verplanck & Hayes, 1953). Thus, there appears to be a reciprocally inhibitory relation between the motivational systems underlying behavior directed toward the acquisition and consumption of food and water.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Previous studies have suggested that an irrelevant drive state reduces the incentive value of the reinforcer, resulting in impairments of acquisition and expression of instrumental behavior (e.g., Capaldi, Hovancik, & Lamb, 1975;Van Hemel & Myer, 1970). In the present study, we examined whether a devaluation process of a food reinforcer induced by a high level of irrelevant thirst (23.5-h water deprivation) would be dependent on incentive learning; i.e., animals' consummatory contact with a food reinforcer under irrelevant thirst.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…A number of studies have observed decreased feeding when animals are deprived of water (e.g., Rolls & McFarland 1973;Verplanck & Hayes, 1953). Parallel to the consummatory response studies, there is extensive literature reporting that instrumental learning is suppressed by the simultaneous presence of an "irrelevant drive": high levels of irrelevant thirst (22-h or more of water deprivation) impaired acquisition (e.g., Bolles & Morlock, 1960;Capaldi, Hovancik, & Lamb, 1975;Kendler, 1945), and expression (e.g., Siegel, 1946;Van Hemel & Myer, 1970) of food-seeking behavior. These results suggest that the food reinforcer is devalued by irrelevant thirst (Capaldi, Hovancik, & Lamb, 1975;Van Hemel & Myer, 1970).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Assuming that water deprivation gives rise to interoceptive stimuli that are different from those produced by food deprivation (cf. Arosel, 1949;Capaldi & Davidson, 1979;Capaldi, Hovancik, & Lamb, 1975;Epstein, 1982;Le Magnen, 1992;Ramachandran & Pearce, 1987), it seems unlikely that discriminative behavior during testing involves control by a configural stimulus that emerges from the combination of food deprivation and contextual cues. There also seems to be no clear a priori basis to assume that stimuli in Context C enter into configural control of responding as the result of simple reinforced training.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%