2014
DOI: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2013.12.034
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Effects of Sleep Restriction on Executive Inhibitory Control and Affect in Young Adults

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

5
59
0
1

Year Published

2015
2015
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 101 publications
(67 citation statements)
references
References 35 publications
5
59
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Sleep disturbance and sleep loss have been observed to be associated with impulsivity. For example, several sleep deprivation studies conducted with young adults have shown sleep loss to be associated with poorer response inhibition using Go/No Go paradigms (e.g., Anderson & Platten, 2011; Drummond, Paulus, & Tapert, 2006), with occasional non-replications (Renn & Cote, 2013; Rossa, Smith, Allan, & Sullivan, 2014). In adults, self-reported measures of disturbed sleep, such as poor sleep quality or insomnia severity, have been associated with self-reported measures of impulsivity (Kamphuis, Dijk, Spreen, & Lancel, 2014; Schmidt, Gay, & Van der Linden, 2008).…”
Section: Other Mechanismsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Sleep disturbance and sleep loss have been observed to be associated with impulsivity. For example, several sleep deprivation studies conducted with young adults have shown sleep loss to be associated with poorer response inhibition using Go/No Go paradigms (e.g., Anderson & Platten, 2011; Drummond, Paulus, & Tapert, 2006), with occasional non-replications (Renn & Cote, 2013; Rossa, Smith, Allan, & Sullivan, 2014). In adults, self-reported measures of disturbed sleep, such as poor sleep quality or insomnia severity, have been associated with self-reported measures of impulsivity (Kamphuis, Dijk, Spreen, & Lancel, 2014; Schmidt, Gay, & Van der Linden, 2008).…”
Section: Other Mechanismsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…To our knowledge, only one study has investigated the impact of short sleep on both impulsive action and impulsive decision-making per se. This study focused on younger adults (aged 18–24 years) after a single night of restricted sleep in which participants were awakened at 4 am compared to their habitual wake time [39]. Greater risk taking behavior on the BART task was observed following restricted sleep, but no differences in performance accuracy on an emotional version of the Go/No-Go were seen [39].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This study focused on younger adults (aged 18–24 years) after a single night of restricted sleep in which participants were awakened at 4 am compared to their habitual wake time [39]. Greater risk taking behavior on the BART task was observed following restricted sleep, but no differences in performance accuracy on an emotional version of the Go/No-Go were seen [39]. Since this is the only prior study to assess the effects of PSD on the Go/No-Go task, it remains unclear whether the differences between this PSD study and the TSD studies are due to differences in the extent of sleep deprivation or to the use of the emotional version of the Go/No-Go task and potential differences in how sleep deprivation impacts the cognitive processes supporting behavioral inhibition versus emotional inhibition.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In another study, chronic sleep restriction (i.e., <6 h) was correlated with reduced vigilance, working memory, and processing speed (Van Dongen, Maislin, Mullington, & Dinges, 2003). Recent work found that restricted sleep (approximately 3 h of sleep) was related to greater propensity toward impulsive risk-taking behavior (Rossa, Smith, Allan, & Sullivan, 2014). …”
mentioning
confidence: 99%