1998
DOI: 10.1017/s014271640001002x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The effects of practicing speech accommodations to older adults

Abstract: This study evaluated the effects of practice with a referential communication task on the form and effectiveness of elderspeak, a speech register targeted at older listeners. The task required the listener to reproduce a route drawn on a map following the speaker's instructions. Young adults were given extended practice with this task to determine if they would modify their fluency, prosody, grammatical complexity, semantic content, or discourse style. The effectiveness of the young speakers' instructions was … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
23
1

Year Published

2001
2001
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
3
3
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 26 publications
(24 citation statements)
references
References 21 publications
0
23
1
Order By: Relevance
“…reported that speech addressed to older audiences (rather than younger ones) was characterized by fewer clauses per utterance, shorter utterances, fewer leftbranching or self-embedded clauses, more lexical fillers (e.g., you know), more sentence fragments, fewer cohesive ties, fewer words of three or more syllables, more repetitions, slower speech rate, and longer pauses. In addition, the use of diminutives was more frequent in addressing dementing than nondementing older adults in this study, but this may be context specific, inasmuch as young speakers in mixed-age dyads rarely use diminutives in referential communication tasks (Kemper et al, 1995(Kemper et al, , 1998c. Evaluations of elderspeak may be moderated by context (e.g.…”
Section: E Language Addressed To Older Adults: Elderspeakmentioning
confidence: 64%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…reported that speech addressed to older audiences (rather than younger ones) was characterized by fewer clauses per utterance, shorter utterances, fewer leftbranching or self-embedded clauses, more lexical fillers (e.g., you know), more sentence fragments, fewer cohesive ties, fewer words of three or more syllables, more repetitions, slower speech rate, and longer pauses. In addition, the use of diminutives was more frequent in addressing dementing than nondementing older adults in this study, but this may be context specific, inasmuch as young speakers in mixed-age dyads rarely use diminutives in referential communication tasks (Kemper et al, 1995(Kemper et al, , 1998c. Evaluations of elderspeak may be moderated by context (e.g.…”
Section: E Language Addressed To Older Adults: Elderspeakmentioning
confidence: 64%
“…Young speakers do not vary speech to older partners in a referential communication task very much regardless of whether the listeners are allowed to interrupt by asking questions or requesting clarification or to provide other feedback (Kemper et al, 1995. However, repeated practice on the same task (Kemper et al, 1998c), listener speech suggesting cognitive impairment (Kemper et al, 1998b), and simulation of speech to adults experiencing cognitive problems rather than living healthy, independent lives in the community (Kemper et al, 1998a) do lead to exaggerated forms of elderspeak in a referential communication task, suggestive of responsiveness to the communicative needs of partners in this task. Older adults also receive more patronizing messages in a simulated persuasion task, especially if targets are described negatively (Hummert et al, 1998).…”
Section: E Language Addressed To Older Adults: Elderspeakmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Kitwood (1997) argues that a person with dementia attracts more malignant social psychology than a person who has not been diagnosed with dementia. Previous research (Kemper et al, 1998) about elderspeak has not reached consensus on whether cognitively impaired older adults are addressed differently than other older adults. The use of interactional data, in this thesis, with two older persons where only one was diagnosed with dementia made it possible to show that the person with dementia was positioned differently than the other older adult in the same conversation.…”
Section: Handling the Dilemma Between Self-determination And Declininmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Elderspeak has been argued to work as a self-fulfilling prophecy where the older person's communicative skills and competence are diminished if the younger conversational partner treats the older person as less competent (Savundranayagam, Ryan, Anas & Orange, 2007). Kemper, Othick, Gerhing, Gubarchuk and Billington (1998) showed that when younger caregivers spontaneously used elderspeak they enhanced the older adults' performance, but when communication became connected to a routine task, the use of patronizing talk became more distinctive, and the older adults perceived themselves as less competent (Kemper et al, 1998).…”
Section: Elderspeakmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This type of speech is known as elder speak or secondary baby talk (Kemper, Otrhick, Gerhing, Gubarchuk, & Billington, 1998;Miller, 2009;Ruscher, 2001;Williams & Nussbaum, 2001). This speech pattern is common when a younger adult speaks to an older adult and is similar to when one speaks to an individual with disabilities (Giles & Gasiorek, 2011;Hummert & Ryan, 2001).…”
Section: Ageismmentioning
confidence: 99%