2012
DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9221.2012.00925.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Effects of Need for Cognition and Need for Affect on Partisan Evaluations

Abstract: The voting behavior literature has advanced two prominent theoretical models of partisanship: the social psychological and rational models. Implicit to both stylized models is the assumption that all partisans process information similarly. Yet, growing research in psychology suggests that individuals possess different motivations when evaluating information. We propose that the applicability of the stylized models of partisanship is conditioned on individuals' need for cognition (NFC) and need for affect (NFA… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1

Citation Types

0
51
0
1

Year Published

2015
2015
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 58 publications
(52 citation statements)
references
References 65 publications
0
51
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Although tested in different ways, partisan motivated reasoning is often evident in analyses of how people process and respond to information in the presence or absence of party endorsements (Bolsen et al 2014;Druckman et al 2013). 2 While much attention is given to individual-level variables that condition the effects of motivations (Arceneaux and Wielen 2013;Lavine et al 2012;Taber and Lodge 2006;Taber et al 2009), research also highlights the moderating role of political contexts. It has been argued that a salient feature of the contemporary American political landscape-elite partisan polarization-alters the influence of partisanship in preference formation in the mass public in at least two ways.…”
Section: Partisan Motivated Reasoning and Elite Polarizationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Although tested in different ways, partisan motivated reasoning is often evident in analyses of how people process and respond to information in the presence or absence of party endorsements (Bolsen et al 2014;Druckman et al 2013). 2 While much attention is given to individual-level variables that condition the effects of motivations (Arceneaux and Wielen 2013;Lavine et al 2012;Taber and Lodge 2006;Taber et al 2009), research also highlights the moderating role of political contexts. It has been argued that a salient feature of the contemporary American political landscape-elite partisan polarization-alters the influence of partisanship in preference formation in the mass public in at least two ways.…”
Section: Partisan Motivated Reasoning and Elite Polarizationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We draw on social identity theory to develop the expressive model and conduct four studies to compare it to an instrumental explanation of campaign involvement. This has generated two competing views of partisanship (Arceneaux and Vander Wielen 2013;Lupu 2013). A series of experiments underscore the power of partisan identity to generate action-oriented emotions that drive campaign activity.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Both models can claim empirical support, and there is growing evidence that instrumental and expressive accounts of partisanship may explain vote choice and public opinion at different times, under differing conditions, and among distinct segments of the electorate (Arceneaux and Vander Wielen 2013;Bullock 2011;Lavine, Johnston, and Steenbergen 2012). Both models can claim empirical support, and there is growing evidence that instrumental and expressive accounts of partisanship may explain vote choice and public opinion at different times, under differing conditions, and among distinct segments of the electorate (Arceneaux and Vander Wielen 2013;Bullock 2011;Lavine, Johnston, and Steenbergen 2012).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Rather than producing the standard statistical significance level of .05 for the probability of a type I error, concluding that two values are statistically different only when their 95 percent confidence intervals do not overlap produces a probability of type I error of approximately .006. These authors recommend using 83.5 percent confidence intervals to achieve type I error rates of 5 percent and recent work in political science has followed their advice (Arceneaux and Vander Wielen ; Lindstadt and Vander Wielen ). We also follow this advice, but use slightly more conservative 85 percent confidence intervals.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%