1981
DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1981.tb03128.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Effects of Differences in Classification Style on Preschool Children's Memory

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
9
0

Year Published

1985
1985
2016
2016

Publication Types

Select...
3

Relationship

1
2

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 3 publications
(9 citation statements)
references
References 16 publications
0
9
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In general, the memory performance of young children on complementary lists has not been very good. This has been the case with lists of experimenter-defined complementary relations (Bjorklund, 1980;Galbraith & Day, 1978) and, also in the case of child-generated groupings, in which high levels of recall and organization are not attained unless subjects are given an opportunity to achieve stable sorting of the to-be-remembered items prior to recall (Bjorklund et al, 1977;Bjorklund & Zaken-Greenberg, 1981;Worden, 1976). This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In general, the memory performance of young children on complementary lists has not been very good. This has been the case with lists of experimenter-defined complementary relations (Bjorklund, 1980;Galbraith & Day, 1978) and, also in the case of child-generated groupings, in which high levels of recall and organization are not attained unless subjects are given an opportunity to achieve stable sorting of the to-be-remembered items prior to recall (Bjorklund et al, 1977;Bjorklund & Zaken-Greenberg, 1981;Worden, 1976). This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Other findings also provide support to age-related differences in children’s cognition. For example, compared to preschool-age children, school-age children are much more likely to use complex cognitive skills to sustain their attention and memory [ 58 61 ]. Such abilities related to memory and attention may help older children engage in more complex conversations with their parents, which in turn may help them attune to their parents’ direction.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, it appears that the less stable organization achieved in the single-sort task was sufficient to result in comparably high levels of performance for children of all SES levels tested here. 3 Previous work contrasting recall performance between single-and criterion-sort tasks (Bjorklund & Zaken-Greenberg, 1981) led us to presume that, relative to the criterion-sort task, minimal organization of items would occur in the single-sort task. Furthermore, it was expected that when subjects organized information on the basis of complementary criteria-a classification style more characteristic of low-SES childrenlevels of recall in the single-sort task would be significantly lower than for subjects who organized information on the basis of taxonomic criteria-a classification style more characteristic of middle-SES children.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For each subject, the percentage of items grouped into exclusively taxonomic categories was computed. For a grouping to be considered exclusively taxonomic, all items in that group, regardless of whether there were two, three, four, or five, had to be from the same experimenter-defined taxonomic category (e.g., cow, dog; carrot, corn, lettuce; see Bjorklund & Zaken-Greenberg, 1981). The percentage of items grouped into exclusively taxonomic categories is presented by SES and task in Table 2.…”
Section: Sorting Datamentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation