1975
DOI: 10.1016/0013-4694(75)90126-1
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The effect of stimulus orientation on the visual evoked potential in human subjects

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
7
1

Year Published

1978
1978
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 15 publications
(8 citation statements)
references
References 17 publications
0
7
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Angular patterns elicit a larger response than do straight-line patterns (Moskowitz et al, 1974;Wu, Ito, Kuwabara, Sugata, & Kida, 1994). An oblique effect in the angular patterns might exist, as was demonstrated for grating patterns (Frost & Kaminer, 1975;Maffei & Campbell, 1970;May & Cullen, 1979;Yoshida et al, 1975). From these findings, it is predicted that the diamond evokes a larger response than the square, because the angular parts of the diamond are located in the horizontal and vertical directions from its center, while those of the square are positioned obliquely.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 72%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Angular patterns elicit a larger response than do straight-line patterns (Moskowitz et al, 1974;Wu, Ito, Kuwabara, Sugata, & Kida, 1994). An oblique effect in the angular patterns might exist, as was demonstrated for grating patterns (Frost & Kaminer, 1975;Maffei & Campbell, 1970;May & Cullen, 1979;Yoshida et al, 1975). From these findings, it is predicted that the diamond evokes a larger response than the square, because the angular parts of the diamond are located in the horizontal and vertical directions from its center, while those of the square are positioned obliquely.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 72%
“…Concerning the effect of spatial orientation upon VEPs, several studies have confirmed the "oblique effect", mainly using grating patterns (Frost & Kaminer, 1975;Maffei & Campbell, 1970;May & Cullen, 1979;Yoshida, Iwahara, & Nagamura, 1975). Appelle (1972) referred to the effect as "a small but consistent superiority in performance when visual stimuli are horizontal or vertical, as opposed to oblique" (p. 266).…”
mentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Proverbio et al (2002) reported orientation-related differences in P1 and P3 components, vertical elicited larger amplitudes than oblique stimuli (they did not look at horizontal stimuli). A study conducted by Yoshida et al (1975) found differences in N1-P2 peak-to-peak amplitude, cardinal stimuli evoked larger responses than oblique stimuli. They obtained waveforms similar to ours using circular black and white gratings as stimuli, a P1-N1-P2 sequence was elicited with peak latencies of 110–120, 180–190, and 270–280 ms, respectively.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…Using event-related potentials (ERP) unequal responses have been obtained to cardinal and oblique orientations in steady state potentials (Maffei and Campbell, 1970; May et al, 1979; Skrandies, 1984; Moskowitz and Sokol, 1985); transient ERPs (Yoshida et al, 1975; Arakawa et al, 2000; Proverbio et al, 2002), and MEG (Koelewijn et al, 2011). …”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In humans, the evidence of a neurophysiological oblique effect bias consists of YEP amplitude and latency (Sokol, Moskowitz, & Hansen, 1989;Zemon,Gutowski, & Horton 1983;Maffei & Campbell, 1970;Yoshida, Iwahara, & Nagamura, 1975) as well as fMRI neuroimaging magnitude (Furmanski and Engel, 2000). Since threshold for a particular stimulus pattern is thought to be directly associated with the number of neurons tuned to that pattern (Essock, Krebs, & Prather, 1997;Anzai, Bearse, & Freeman, 1995 ), this numerical anisotropy in the tuning preferences of neurons would explain the presence of the behavioral oblique effect.…”
Section: The Physiological Oblique Effectmentioning
confidence: 99%