2015
DOI: 10.1177/0305735615577248
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The effect of repetition on preference ratings for select unfamiliar musical examples: Does preference transfer?

Abstract: The purpose of this research was to examine the effects of repeated exposure to select musical examples on participants' preference ratings, and to investigate the potential transfer of preference to similar, but unfamiliar musical examples. A pre-test-post-test nonequivalent control group design was used to assess preference ratings before and after a program of repeated exposure. A second post-test was included to assess preference ratings for similar, but unfamiliar examples, in order to examine the possibi… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

1
14
2

Year Published

2016
2016
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 11 publications
(17 citation statements)
references
References 36 publications
1
14
2
Order By: Relevance
“…Interestingly, while familiarity ratings showed a steady, significant increase over subsequent exposures for both stimuli (as expected), no significant relationship was observed between preference and familiarity. This runs counter to the general findings that preference follows the segments of the inverted-U as a function of familiarity (e.g., Bartlett, 1973; Bradley, 1971; Getz, 1966; Hargreaves, 1987; Hunter & Schellenberg, 2011; Johnston, 2015; North & Hargreaves, 1995, 1997; Schellenberg, Peretz, & Vieillard, 2008; Verveer, Barry, & Bousfield, 1933; Washburn, Child, & Abel, 1927). Similarly, this floor-effect result cannot be explained by the mere exposure effect (Zajonc, 1968), an important alternative to the inverted-U model (Hargreaves & North, 2010).…”
Section: Discussioncontrasting
confidence: 75%
“…Interestingly, while familiarity ratings showed a steady, significant increase over subsequent exposures for both stimuli (as expected), no significant relationship was observed between preference and familiarity. This runs counter to the general findings that preference follows the segments of the inverted-U as a function of familiarity (e.g., Bartlett, 1973; Bradley, 1971; Getz, 1966; Hargreaves, 1987; Hunter & Schellenberg, 2011; Johnston, 2015; North & Hargreaves, 1995, 1997; Schellenberg, Peretz, & Vieillard, 2008; Verveer, Barry, & Bousfield, 1933; Washburn, Child, & Abel, 1927). Similarly, this floor-effect result cannot be explained by the mere exposure effect (Zajonc, 1968), an important alternative to the inverted-U model (Hargreaves & North, 2010).…”
Section: Discussioncontrasting
confidence: 75%
“…Statistical analysis revealed a significant negative effect of stimulus repetition on likability and no interaction effect of repetition with information content. This result documents the independence of structural and veridical predictability, and it contrasts with earlier findings in which stimulus repetition was positively correlated with likability (Johnston, 2016). Data from both experiments by Gold et al (2019a) also showed stronger Wundt effects (i.e., higher skewness and kurtosis values) in participants with higher musical perceptual abilities.…”
contrasting
confidence: 52%
“…Perhaps the most prominent difference between the two modes of vocalization is the absence of beat-based regularity in speech and its dominance in ID singing (Longhi, 2009;Nakata & Trehub, 2011) and music in general (Savage, Brown, Sakai, & Currie, 2015). Although ID speech incorporates more repetition than adult-directed speech, it lacks the extensive repetition that characterizes music, a feature that adds to its appeal and memorability (Johnston, 2016;Margulis, 2013Margulis, , 2014. Stereotypy and repetition in ID singing may contribute to its efficacy in regulating infant arousal (Cirelli, Jurewicz, & Trehub, 2019;Shenfield, Trehub, & Nakata, 2003) and its greater efficacy than ID speech in delaying the onset of infant distress (Corbeil, Trehub, & Peretz, 2016).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%