2022
DOI: 10.1016/j.lmot.2022.101812
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The effect of partial and continuous reinforcement on the generalization of conditioned fear in humans

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

4
16
0

Year Published

2022
2022
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
3
3

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 9 publications
(21 citation statements)
references
References 76 publications
4
16
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Surprisingly, no effect of the reinforcement rate was evident in SCR during acquisition and generalization despite some evidence that SCR is modulated by US prediction (de Berker et al, 2016;Ojala & Bach, 2020). However, Zhao et al (2022) used a 50% reinforcement schedule for all groups in generalization which matched the reinforcement schedule of one group (i.e., the 50% group) during acquisition. This means that each group experienced a different reduction (while none for the 50%) of CS-US contingency, making the generalization test difficult to compare across groups.…”
Section: No Influence Of Threat Uncertainty On Fear Generalizationmentioning
confidence: 98%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Surprisingly, no effect of the reinforcement rate was evident in SCR during acquisition and generalization despite some evidence that SCR is modulated by US prediction (de Berker et al, 2016;Ojala & Bach, 2020). However, Zhao et al (2022) used a 50% reinforcement schedule for all groups in generalization which matched the reinforcement schedule of one group (i.e., the 50% group) during acquisition. This means that each group experienced a different reduction (while none for the 50%) of CS-US contingency, making the generalization test difficult to compare across groups.…”
Section: No Influence Of Threat Uncertainty On Fear Generalizationmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Despite the inherent uncertainty associated with partial reinforcement rates, studies on fear generalization use various reinforcement schedules during acquisition, ranging from 33% (Morey et al, 2015) to 75% or even 100% (Lemmens et al, 2021;Lissek et al, 2010) making it difficult to compare. To our knowledge the only study that directly investigated the effect of partial and continuous reinforcement schedules on fear generalization is the one by Zhao et al (2022). The authors compared three groups with reinforcement schedules of 50%, 75% and 100% in acquisition and found overall increased generalization magnitudes for threat expectancy ratings for the groups with partial (50% and 75%) reinforcement while the continuous reinforcement group showed a less steep generalization gradient.…”
Section: No Influence Of Threat Uncertainty On Fear Generalizationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…When the values of episode and t are very large, the constant term in the T(episode,t) function and the coefficients of T and N have negligible effects on episode and t. At the same time, we should note that the main influencing factor of the T (episode,t) function is T � N, then the time complexity of the algorithm used in this study is shown in Equation (25).…”
Section: Algorithm Time Complexity Calculationmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…As mentioned above, the generalisation ability of reinforcement learning affects the deployment of reinforcement learning models in the real world. Therefore, how to improve the generalisation ability of the reinforcement model is an important issue for the application of reinforcement learning in air combat decision‐making [25].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…It is important to note, however, that all of the aforementioned studies concurrently varied the threat reinforcement rate and the amount of learning experience in their experimental designs. That is, higher threat reinforcement rates corresponded to proportionally higher numbers of CS-US pairings, with Dunsmoor et al (2007) using reinforcement rates (and # of CS-US trials) of 50% (20) and 100% (40), Grady et al (2016) using 50% (20), 50%-100% (30), 100%-50% (30), and 100% (40), Zhao et al (2022) using 50% (4), 75% (6) and 100% (8) and Chin et al (2016) using 50% (4) and 75% (6). This is important because more learning experience could lead to enhanced threat discrimination by reducing estimation uncertainty (Payzan-LeNestour et al, 2013), thus this feature of the experimental design challenges direct attributions of enhanced threat discrimination learning to reinforcement rate alone.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%