2011
DOI: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.06.007
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The effect of language cues on infants’ representational flexibility in a deferred imitation task

Abstract: Twelve-and 15-month-old infants who received simple verbal cues at encoding and retrieval exhibited superior representational flexibility on an imitation task compared to infants who did not receive those cues. Verbal cues can help early-verbal infants overcome perceptual dissimilarity and express knowledge in novel situations. The effect of language cues on infants' representational flexibility in a deferred imitation task Jane S. Herbert University of Sheffield, Department of Psychology, Western Bank, Sheffi… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
2

Citation Types

0
25
0

Year Published

2012
2012
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

3
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 19 publications
(26 citation statements)
references
References 17 publications
0
25
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Prior research using multiple paradigms suggests that infants are successful in extracting and mapping linguistic labels early in the second year of life (e.g., Barr & Wyss, 2008; Ganea et al , 2008; Herbert, 2011; Herbert & Hayne, 2000; Kay‐Raining Bird & Chapman, 1998; Pruden et al , 2006; Simcock et al , 2011; Waxman, 2008). The addition of specific linguistic cues (nonsense names, object labels, verbs) might, therefore, facilitate transfer of action imitation across dimension.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Prior research using multiple paradigms suggests that infants are successful in extracting and mapping linguistic labels early in the second year of life (e.g., Barr & Wyss, 2008; Ganea et al , 2008; Herbert, 2011; Herbert & Hayne, 2000; Kay‐Raining Bird & Chapman, 1998; Pruden et al , 2006; Simcock et al , 2011; Waxman, 2008). The addition of specific linguistic cues (nonsense names, object labels, verbs) might, therefore, facilitate transfer of action imitation across dimension.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For example, providing infants with additional encoding time (Barr, Dowden & Hayne, 1996), the opportunity to immediately reproduce target actions (Hayne, Barr & Herbert, 2003), language cues (Bauer, Wenner, Dropik & Wewerka, 2000; Hayne & Herbert, 2004), or allowing an association to be formed between the target event and another, longer remembered, event (Barr, Vieira & Rovee‐Collier, 2001, 2002) can protract the duration of retention for the target actions in an imitation task. Similarly, the opportunity to immediately reproduce the target actions (Hayne et al , 2003; Learmonth, Lamberth & Rovee‐Collier, 2004, 2005), and the provision of language cues at encoding and retrieval (Herbert & Hayne, 2000; Herbert, 2011), can facilitate infants’ ability to retrieve their memories in situations that are similar, but not identical, to those experienced at encoding.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In contrast to the facilitation observed at 21-months, the presence or absence of naming information did not appear to influence infants’ performance on the extension trial at 18-months. This was unexpected, given that previous research has documented the facilitative effects of naming in other types of tasks, as well as with even younger infants (e.g., Booth and Waxman, 2002, 2003; Graham et al, 2004; Keates and Graham, 2008; Waxman, 2008; Herbert, 2011). The lack of facilitation reported here likely resulted from two factors.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 83%