2013
DOI: 10.1037/a0028507
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The effect of forced-choice questions on children's suggestibility: A comparison of multiple-choice and yes/no questions.

Abstract: Previous research has suggested that, when interviewing young children, responses to yes/no questions are less reliable than responses to multiple-choice questions (Peterson & Grant, 2001). However, according to fuzzy trace theory, some forms of multiple-choice questions should elicit higher error rates than yes/no questions. Fuzzy trace theory is a theory of cognitive development that suggests there are two types of memory traces: Verbatim traces include exact details of an experience, whereas gist traces rep… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

4
46
0
1

Year Published

2013
2013
2018
2018

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 33 publications
(51 citation statements)
references
References 66 publications
4
46
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…When asked yes/no questions, they tend to provide unelaborated ‘yes’ and ‘no’ responses (Stolzenberg & Lyon, 2014). When asked forced-choice questions, they tend to simply choose one of the responses, even when neither response is correct (Peterson & Grant, 2001; Rocha, Marche, & Briere, 2013). Even when they don’t know the answer, they will answer yes/no and forced-choice questions rather than give don’t know responses (Memon & Vartoukian, 1996; Poole & Lindsay, 2001; Rudy & Goodman, 1991).…”
Section: The Significance Of Question Type: Misunderstanding and Undementioning
confidence: 99%
“…When asked yes/no questions, they tend to provide unelaborated ‘yes’ and ‘no’ responses (Stolzenberg & Lyon, 2014). When asked forced-choice questions, they tend to simply choose one of the responses, even when neither response is correct (Peterson & Grant, 2001; Rocha, Marche, & Briere, 2013). Even when they don’t know the answer, they will answer yes/no and forced-choice questions rather than give don’t know responses (Memon & Vartoukian, 1996; Poole & Lindsay, 2001; Rudy & Goodman, 1991).…”
Section: The Significance Of Question Type: Misunderstanding and Undementioning
confidence: 99%
“…As Rocha et al . () point out, however, the ‘something else’ option has not been empirically tested. Therefore, it is unknown whether children often (or appropriately) choose the ‘something else’ option.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Children's formal reticence suggests that their misleading answers to questions about clothing will extend to forced-choice questions, that is questions that provide options conjoined with 'or', such as 'Were your clothes on off?'). When asked forced-choice questions, young children tend to choose one of the responses, even when neither response is correct (Peterson & Grant, 2001;Rocha, Marche, & Briere, 2013). Children exhibit these response tendencies even when they do not know the answers to the questions; that is, they are disinclined to provide 'I don't know' answers to yes/no and forced-choice questions (Memon & Vartoukian, 1996;Poole & Lindsay, 2001;Rudy & Goodman, 1991).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The use of multiple choice questions limited the ability of the questionnaire to capture the complexity of occupation as it reduced the correct response to a single best answer 17,37 . Some questions were leading in nature, and responses may therefore not have been a true reflection of participants' knowledge and attitudes 37 . A further concern arose during the marking process, where the expectancy effect 38 may have influenced the reliability of scores assigned by raters.…”
Section: Strengths and Limitationsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Time constraints did not permit piloting of the final questionnaire and therefore it is not known whether the test-retest reliability for section C improved after revisions were made following the pilot study. The use of multiple choice questions limited the ability of the questionnaire to capture the complexity of occupation as it reduced the correct response to a single best answer 17,37 . Some questions were leading in nature, and responses may therefore not have been a true reflection of participants' knowledge and attitudes 37 .…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%