2002
DOI: 10.1111/1467-7687.00242
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The effect of early visual deprivation on the development of face processing

Abstract: We evaluated the importance of early visual input for the later development of expertise in face processing by studying 17 patients, aged 10 to 38 years, treated for bilateral congenital cataracts that deprived them of patterned visual input for the first 7 weeks or more after birth. We administered five computerized tasks that required matching faces on the basis of identity (with changed facial expression or head orientation), facial expression, gaze direction and lip reading. Compared to an age-matched cont… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

3
46
1

Year Published

2007
2007
2017
2017

Publication Types

Select...
5
3

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 106 publications
(53 citation statements)
references
References 76 publications
3
46
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Support for this reasoning can be found in a study of patients with congenital cataracts who were deprived of patterned visual input in their first months of life. When tested years after deprivation, these patients were able to normally process featural information from both faces and geometric patterns, but suffered from impaired processing of configural information specific to faces (e.g., distance between the eyes; see Le Grand, Mondloch, Maurer, & Brent, 2001; Geldart, Mondloch, Maurer, de Schonen, & Brent, 2002; Nelson et al, 2006). Studies of the “other species effect” and “other race effect” also demonstrate that typically developing infants retain the ability to discriminate types of faces to which they have been exposed, but lose the ability to identify other types (Pascalis et al, 2005; Kelly et al, 2007).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Support for this reasoning can be found in a study of patients with congenital cataracts who were deprived of patterned visual input in their first months of life. When tested years after deprivation, these patients were able to normally process featural information from both faces and geometric patterns, but suffered from impaired processing of configural information specific to faces (e.g., distance between the eyes; see Le Grand, Mondloch, Maurer, & Brent, 2001; Geldart, Mondloch, Maurer, de Schonen, & Brent, 2002; Nelson et al, 2006). Studies of the “other species effect” and “other race effect” also demonstrate that typically developing infants retain the ability to discriminate types of faces to which they have been exposed, but lose the ability to identify other types (Pascalis et al, 2005; Kelly et al, 2007).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Geldart et al (2002) investigated a group of 17 patients with a history of bilateral deprivation and found that identification of faces was impaired when the stimuli were presented with different head positions or expressions. However, matching of expressions and the ability to lip read were unimpaired (Geldart et al, 2002).…”
Section: Global Deficits In Amblyopiamentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Geldart et al (2002) investigated a group of 17 patients with a history of bilateral deprivation and found that identification of faces was impaired when the stimuli were presented with different head positions or expressions. However, matching of expressions and the ability to lip read were unimpaired (Geldart et al, 2002). A subsequent study corroborated the deficit in varying head position and lighting conditions and revealed a deficit for lip reading using a more complex task (Putzar et al, 2010b).…”
Section: Global Deficits In Amblyopiamentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Face detection can, therefore, develop normally even in the absence of early visual input. Such patients are also as good as controls at matching faces based on the direction of eye gaze, facial expression, or sound being mouthed, at least for the gross types of discrimination that were tested, which likely require only featural processing (Geldart, Mondloch, Maurer, de Schonen, & Brent, 2002). They are also normal at matching individual faces based on the shape of their internal features or of their external contour (Le Grand, Mondloch, Maurer, & Brent, 2001;Mondloch et al, 2003), even when the feature differences include ones that are hard to detect for adults with normal vision (Mondloch, Robbins, & Maurer, 2010).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…The results from cataract-reversal patients were compared to those of 24 age-matched controls with normal vision. Based on previous findings, we predicted that patients would be less sensitive than controls to the composite illusion (Le Grand et al, 2004), to feature spacing in upright human faces (Le Grand et al, 2001;Robbins et al, 2010) and to the identity of an unfamiliar face presented from different viewpoints (Geldart et al, 2002). Because these perceptual deficits match those reported in acquired prosopagnosia, we also suspected that patients would be significantly impaired in the tests of face memory (Famous Faces Task, Cambridge Face Memory test) despite the absence of complaints about their face recognition abilities (Prosopagnosic Questionnaire).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%