2016
DOI: 10.1111/bij.12887
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The effect of distastefulness and conspicuous coloration on the post-attack rejection behaviour of predators and survival of prey

Abstract: Aposematic insects use bright colours and/or distinct markings to advertise their toxins to potential predators. When toxins are bitter-tasting and detectable upon attack, birds are able to use taste when making decisions about whether or not to eat defended prey. Taste-rejection behaviour, when birds taste but do not ingest a prey item, is often assumed to increase the survival of defended prey, yet few empirical studies have investigated the post-attack survival rates of live defended insects. We used na€ ıv… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
9
0

Year Published

2019
2019
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6
1
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 10 publications
(10 citation statements)
references
References 61 publications
0
9
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Common terrestrial [61][62][63][64] and marine [65] generalist predators are capable of taste-rejecting prey on the basis of their chemical defences, as well as discriminating among the visual features of signals [66][67][68]. Conspicuousness and defences have also been shown to interact to increase the probability of prey rejection [69,70], in part through a learned wariness toward conspicuous prey [71]. The taxonomic and ecological breadth of this work suggests 'go-slow' sampling as an appealing catch-all explanation, in keeping with the generality of our findings (table 2).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Common terrestrial [61][62][63][64] and marine [65] generalist predators are capable of taste-rejecting prey on the basis of their chemical defences, as well as discriminating among the visual features of signals [66][67][68]. Conspicuousness and defences have also been shown to interact to increase the probability of prey rejection [69,70], in part through a learned wariness toward conspicuous prey [71]. The taxonomic and ecological breadth of this work suggests 'go-slow' sampling as an appealing catch-all explanation, in keeping with the generality of our findings (table 2).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…taste rejected). This means that white moths are protected at the last line of defence and may rely more on taste rejection by predators (Skelhorn and Rowe 2006a, b;Halpin and Rowe 2017), which could contribute to balancing the fitness of the two morphs in the wild.…”
Section: Predator Deterrence At Long Distancementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Therefore, it is reasonable to expect that in this study, more mimics were eaten than models after they were attacked because taste sampling allows predators to discriminate between prey with ambiguous warning signals using taste. We also did not estimate prey survival, but previous studies show that taste sampled prey can often survive predator attacks [37][38][39][40] and defended prey may be attacked with less force than undefended prey [21,41,42]. Moreover, aposematic species often evolve tough and exible bodies [43][44][45] meaning that models may have higher survival than mimics [3].…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%