1979
DOI: 10.1080/10862967909547324
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Effect of Background Knowledge on Young Children's Comprehension of Explicit and Implicit Information

Abstract: Abstract. To investigate the applicability of schema-theoretic notions to young children's comprehension of textually explicit and inferrable information, slightly above-average second grade readers with strong and weak schemata for knowledge about spiders read a passage about spiders and answered wh-questions tapping both explicitly stated information and knowledge that necessarily had to be inferred from the text. Main effects were found for strength of prior knowledge [p < .01), and question type (p < .01).… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

7
142
3
8

Year Published

1983
1983
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
7
1
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 261 publications
(162 citation statements)
references
References 5 publications
7
142
3
8
Order By: Relevance
“…Average readers with high prior knowledge have been found to score significantly better than readers with low prior knowledge on scriptally implicit questions but not explicit questions (Pearson et al, 1979). Thus, background information may have increased the readers' depth of processing and their ability to process the more difficult type of questions.…”
mentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Average readers with high prior knowledge have been found to score significantly better than readers with low prior knowledge on scriptally implicit questions but not explicit questions (Pearson et al, 1979). Thus, background information may have increased the readers' depth of processing and their ability to process the more difficult type of questions.…”
mentioning
confidence: 98%
“…For instance, the second graders in Pearson, Hansen, and Gordon (1979) could be categorized as snake experts or novices. Questions on a short textabout snakes dealt with information explicitly presented in text, as weil as facts that were only implied in text, but could be deduced based on prior knowledge.…”
Section: The Knowledge Basementioning
confidence: 99%
“…When people poss t1S baekground knowledge that is eonsistent with the eontent of the text, there is plenty of reason to expect that prior knowledge aetivation should increase learning, for at a minimum the reader is thinking "deeply" about the topie of the upcoming text (ef., Riekards, 1976;Riekards & Divesta, 1974;Watts & Anderson, 1971). Not suprisingly, people do remernher more that they read if they possess and have aetivated relevant knowledge (e.g., Anderson & Pearson, 1984;.Arnold & Brooks, 1976;Bransford & Johnson, 1972;Pearson et al, 1979;Tierney & Cunningham, 1984). But what ifthe person does not possess relevant prior knowledge, andin faet, has misconeeptions about the topic eovered in the text?…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This advice is an attempt to translate into practice important research findings on the role of background knowledge (Beck & Carpenter, 1986;Beck, McCaslin, & McKeown, 1981;Daneman, 1991;McKeown, Beck, Sinatra, & Loxterman, 1992;Pearson, Hansen, & Gordon, 1979;Roller, 1990;Spilich, Vesonder, Chiesi, & Voss, 1979). These sorts of prereading interventions tend to be conceptualized in one of three ways (Tierney & Cunningham, 1984): (a) as an attempt to teach vocabulary, that is, crucial word meanings before the words are encountered in text; (b) as an effort to enrich background knowledge, that is, knowledge relevant to understanding the text from the teachers' or curriculum developers' view; and (c) as an encouragement to use analogy, that is, to use sets of information that have similar attributes to the information judged relevant to understanding the upcoming text.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%