2010
DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00931.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Ebbinghaus illusion deceives adults but not young children

Abstract: The sensitivity of size perception to context has been used to distinguish between ‘vision for action’ and ‘vision for perception’, and to study cultural, psychopathological, and developmental differences in perception. The status of that evidence is much debated, however. Here we use a rigorous double dissociation paradigm based on the Ebbinghaus illusion, and find that for children below 7 years of age size discrimination is much less affected by surround size. Young children are less accurate than adults wh… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

7
118
0

Year Published

2010
2010
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
5
3
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 117 publications
(125 citation statements)
references
References 33 publications
7
118
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Here, the width and height of the stimulus serve as contrasting elements, in the same way that the large and small circles do in the Ebbinghaus illusion, with the smaller width, for example, inducing the height to look larger by contrast. However, unlike the Ebbinghaus illusion, which requires sensitivity to global context (de Fockert, Davidoff, Fagot, Parron, & Goldstein, 2007;Doherty, Campbell, Tsuji, & Phillips, 2010), in this case the contrasting "elements" are integral dimensions of the same object.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Here, the width and height of the stimulus serve as contrasting elements, in the same way that the large and small circles do in the Ebbinghaus illusion, with the smaller width, for example, inducing the height to look larger by contrast. However, unlike the Ebbinghaus illusion, which requires sensitivity to global context (de Fockert, Davidoff, Fagot, Parron, & Goldstein, 2007;Doherty, Campbell, Tsuji, & Phillips, 2010), in this case the contrasting "elements" are integral dimensions of the same object.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This perceptual task is as similar as possible to the priming task to allow for meaningful conclusions from a potential dissociation between the results in both tasks (Schmidt & Vorberg, 2006). It also represents the method of forced-choice discrimination which is an established method to measure the perceptual strength of visual illusions (e.g., Doherty, Campbell, Tsuji & Phillips, 2010;Moore & Egeth, 1997;Reynolds, 1978).…”
Section: Response Priming As a Tool To Investigate The Time Course Ofmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Note that in this condition, if subjects choose the array with larger surrounds then they will be wrong on every trial. As in prior studies, only 16 trials were presented in the helpful condition, and they were all at the hardest difficulty level (for Rutgers .05° size difference between center circles) (Phillips et al, 2004; Doherty et al, 2008; Doherty, 2010). The 96 trials in the context conditions (80 in the misleading and 16 in the helpful conditions) were presented in a different random order for each subject.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The effect has been known for over 100 years (Titchener, 1902), and has been the subject of numerous experiments, especially since the 1970s (e.g., Massaro and Anderson, 1971; Girgus et al, 1972; Weintraub and Schneck, 1986; Coren and Enns, 1993; Rose and Bressan, 2002; Doherty, 2010; Schwarzkopf and Rees, 2013). Patients with schizophrenia have demonstrated reduced illusion effects, expressed as more accurate size perception compared to controls when judging target circle size in misleading context conditions (Uhlhaas et al, 2006; Silverstein et al, 2013; Tibber et al, 2013).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%