Media Exposure During Infancy and Early Childhood 2016
DOI: 10.1007/978-3-319-45102-2_3
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Dimensional Divide: Learning from TV and Touchscreens During Early Childhood

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

0
35
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
4
3
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 70 publications
(36 citation statements)
references
References 80 publications
0
35
0
Order By: Relevance
“…While age‐related changes in learning may be explained partly by differences in learning strategies, they may also be due to changes in cognitive constraints . If the cognitive load created by interactivity exceeds available resources, interactivity may hinder learning .…”
Section: Who Is Most Likely To Benefit From Interactivity?mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…While age‐related changes in learning may be explained partly by differences in learning strategies, they may also be due to changes in cognitive constraints . If the cognitive load created by interactivity exceeds available resources, interactivity may hinder learning .…”
Section: Who Is Most Likely To Benefit From Interactivity?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, the specific conditions that lead to learning are unclear: Not all types of interactivity increase learning and not all children learn to the same degree. The extent to which young children learn from screen media depends on (among other things) the intersection between the cognitive demands of a particular learning task and each child's cognitive resources . For instance, interactive features that guide attention to important information may be more useful than those that divert attention from that information or provide little guidance.…”
Section: Where Do We Go From Here?mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…What is even less clear is how children's EF is affected by screen exposure in the first 6 months of life. Young infants only view screens for 3-5 s at a time (for summary, see Kirkorian, Pempek, & Choi, 2017), and child-directed programming does not seem to be understood by until age two (Anderson & Subrahmanyam, 2017;Hipp et al, 2017). Before this, all television content can be understood as background television, especially in the very early months when parents are likely to be watching adult-directed content around their infants (Anderson & Subrahmanyam, 2017).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Across a wide range of tasks, toddlers and young children show decrements in learning when there is a change in the physical context between the learning and testing environments (Barnett & Ceci, ; Hartshorn et al ., ; Herbert & Hayne, ; Strouse & Ganea, ; Strouse & Troseth, ). Specifically, toddlers demonstrate this transfer deficit (for reviews see Barr, , ; Hipp et al ., ) when transferring from video (Dickerson, Gerhardstein, Zack, & Barr, ; Moser et al ., ; Nielsen, Simcock, & Jenkins, ) and touchscreens (Moser et al ., ; Zack, Gerhardstein, Meltzoff, & Barr, ; Zimmermann, Moser, Lee, Gerhardstein, & Barr, ) to real‐world settings. This transfer deficit is bidirectional (Moser et al ., ; Zack et al ., ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%