2018
DOI: 10.1111/desc.12674
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The development of fast‐mapping and novel word retention strategies in monolingual and bilingual infants

Abstract: The mutual exclusivity (ME) assumption is proposed to facilitate early word learning by guiding infants to map novel words to novel referents. This study assessed the emergence and use of ME to both disambiguate and retain the meanings of novel words across development in 18-month-old monolingual and bilingual children (Experiment 1; N = 58), and in a sub-group of these children again at 24 months of age (Experiment 2: N = 32). Both monolinguals and bilinguals employed ME to select the referent of a novel labe… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

11
51
1

Year Published

2018
2018
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

2
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 37 publications
(63 citation statements)
references
References 50 publications
11
51
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Test-baseline difference scores were used as indices of word learning whereby a larger positive score denoted an infant's greater tendency to look at the target after hearing its label (see Byers-Heinlein & Werker, 2013;Kalashnikova, Mattock, & Monaghan, 2016;Kalashnikova, Escudero, & Kidd, 2018 for use of difference scores in similar analyses). NAR infants were reported to produce an average of 55.63 (SD = 55.56, range 9-191) words and AR infants 54.10 words (SD = 60.63, range 4-189), and there was no statistical difference between the two groups' scores, t(19) = .061, p = .952, d = .028.…”
Section: Ar and Nar Infants' Performance In The Test Trials Is Depictmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Test-baseline difference scores were used as indices of word learning whereby a larger positive score denoted an infant's greater tendency to look at the target after hearing its label (see Byers-Heinlein & Werker, 2013;Kalashnikova, Mattock, & Monaghan, 2016;Kalashnikova, Escudero, & Kidd, 2018 for use of difference scores in similar analyses). NAR infants were reported to produce an average of 55.63 (SD = 55.56, range 9-191) words and AR infants 54.10 words (SD = 60.63, range 4-189), and there was no statistical difference between the two groups' scores, t(19) = .061, p = .952, d = .028.…”
Section: Ar and Nar Infants' Performance In The Test Trials Is Depictmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Correlational analyses were then conducted between the infants' vocabulary scores and their word-learning scores. Test-baseline difference scores were used as indices of word learning whereby a larger positive score denoted an infant's greater tendency to look at the target after hearing its label (see Byers-Heinlein & Werker, 2013;Kalashnikova, Mattock, & Monaghan, 2016;Kalashnikova, Escudero, & Kidd, 2018 for use of difference scores in similar analyses). This analysis revealed a significant correlation, r(21) = .466, p = .033, demonstrating that even though it was not the case that AR infants had smaller expressive vocabularies as a group, infants with larger vocabulary sizes were significantly more likely to look at the labelled target in the test phase of our word-learning task, indicative of greater learning.…”
Section: Ar and Nar Infants' Performance In The Test Trials Is Depictmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In contrast, in reaching versions of the tasks eighteen-month-old toddlers do not perform well on reference selection or retention (Kucker, McMurry & Samuelson, 2018), 24-month-olds perform well on reference selection, but they do not retain the mappings (Samuelson & Horst, 2008), and by 30 months of age children are good at both (see Kucker, McMurray, & Samuelson, 2015a for a review and discussion). Both reference selection and retention, however, can be influenced by a number of external and organismic factors (see e.g., Axelsson & Horst, 2014;Kalashnikova, Escudero & Kidd, 2018;Kucker & Samuelson, 2012;Pomper & Saffran, 2018, Twomey, Ranson, & Horst, 2013. Still other research indicates that children as young as 13 months of age can map and, in some cases, retain novel word-object mappings when only one name and one object are presented at a time (Schafer & Plunkett, 1998;Woodward, Markman, & Fitzsimmons, 1994).…”
Section: Reference Selection and Retention Tasksmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Previous research indicates that early linguistic experience can impact the extent to which children employ ME as a default word-learning strategy in their second year of life (Byers-Heinlein & Werker, 2009;2013;Davidson et al, 1997;Kalashnikova et al, 2016bKalashnikova et al, , 2018Kandhadai et al, 2017). However, the aspects of early language experience that lead to successful acceptance of lexical overlap by both monolingual and bilingual children remain unclear.…”
Section: Acceptance Of Lexical Overlapmentioning
confidence: 99%