2000
DOI: 10.1006/jecp.2000.2564
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The Development of Automaticity in Accessing Number Magnitude

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

30
273
12
2

Year Published

2002
2002
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6
2
1

Relationship

1
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 232 publications
(317 citation statements)
references
References 22 publications
30
273
12
2
Order By: Relevance
“…This means that a prerequisite for the automatic triggering of this procedure is the capacity to automatically convert digits into their analogical representations. However, it has been shown that, although the Arabic code is easily learnt and used by young children, the automatic access to analogue representations from digits does not seem to occur before age 9 or 10 (Girelli, Lucangeli, & Butterworth, 2000;Rubinsten, Henik, Berger, & Shahar-Shalev, 2002). This would explain why the developmental shift from algorithmic computing to direct retrieval has been described as occurring around these ages (Ashcraft & Fierman, 1982).…”
Section: Acquisitionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This means that a prerequisite for the automatic triggering of this procedure is the capacity to automatically convert digits into their analogical representations. However, it has been shown that, although the Arabic code is easily learnt and used by young children, the automatic access to analogue representations from digits does not seem to occur before age 9 or 10 (Girelli, Lucangeli, & Butterworth, 2000;Rubinsten, Henik, Berger, & Shahar-Shalev, 2002). This would explain why the developmental shift from algorithmic computing to direct retrieval has been described as occurring around these ages (Ashcraft & Fierman, 1982).…”
Section: Acquisitionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Moreover, the distance and the size effects are believed to reflect a process that is derived from the mental number line (Moyer and Landauer, 1967). Hence, additional support for the suggestion that different dimensions interfere with each other as a result of a shared magnitude processing is given by the interaction of the size congruity effect with the distance effect (Cohen Kadosh and Henik, 2006;Cohen Kadosh et al, 2007e;Girelli et al, 2000;Henik and Tzelgov, 1982;Schwarz and Heinze, 1998;Schwarz and Ischebeck, 2003;Tzelgov et al, 1992), and the size effect (Pinhas et al, submitted for publication).…”
Section: Similar Effect Patterns With Different Kinds Of Quantity: Thmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the size congruity paradigm, children show a mutual interference between size and number starting at the end of first grade when they have to perform a magnitude comparison (Rubinsten et al, 2002). However, such a mutual interference is absent at the beginning of first grade (Girelli et al, 2000;Rubinsten et al, 2002). These results reflect the slow development of the automatic association between digits and the quantity they refer to (see Ansari et al, 2005, for the evolution of the neural basis of digits comparison in childhood).…”
Section: Evidence For Shared Mechanisms and Representations In Childrenmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Evidence of shared magnitude processing arises when the distance effects of different magnitudes interact. Among the most investigated is the interaction between physical and symbolic size, as depicted by the numerical Stroop paradigm (Girelli, Lucangeli, & Butterworth, 2000;Henik & Tzelgov, 1982). In this paradigm, participants are required to indicate either the numerically or physically larger digit (relevant dimension) within a pair, while the other (irrelevant) dimension varies, yielding congruent (e.g., 2 ) or incongruent (e.g., 4) conditions.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%