1985
DOI: 10.3758/bf03200014
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The CS-US interval (ISI) function in rabbit nictitating membrane response conditioning with very long intertriai intervals

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
14
0

Year Published

1994
1994
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
4
4
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 25 publications
(14 citation statements)
references
References 23 publications
0
14
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The best ISI for promoting CRs is not a fixed point in time; it differs with the response, the intensity and nature of CS and US, and the temporal distribution of CS-US pairings (Atkins et al 1994, Berk & Miller 1978, Lennartz & Weinberger 1992, Schneiderman 1972. Moreover, lengthening the intertrial interval (ITI) raises the optimal ISI (Gibbon et al 1977, Levinthal et al 1985, which suggests that the key variable is the ISI/ITI ratio.…”
Section: Simple Acquisition With a Single Cue Or Responsementioning
confidence: 99%
“…The best ISI for promoting CRs is not a fixed point in time; it differs with the response, the intensity and nature of CS and US, and the temporal distribution of CS-US pairings (Atkins et al 1994, Berk & Miller 1978, Lennartz & Weinberger 1992, Schneiderman 1972. Moreover, lengthening the intertrial interval (ITI) raises the optimal ISI (Gibbon et al 1977, Levinthal et al 1985, which suggests that the key variable is the ISI/ITI ratio.…”
Section: Simple Acquisition With a Single Cue Or Responsementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Our analyses, presented above, show that shock level was stronger for Supp than for NM, particularly for the Levinthal et al (1985) studies. This is based on coulombs.…”
Section: Us Intensity: Higher Levels Of Shock Current In Nm Conditioningmentioning
confidence: 51%
“…Two ofthe seven (Salafia et al, 1976;Salafia et al, 1975) did not report trials to the 1st CR, but did report trials to the 20th CR, and therefore could not be used in the survey. Two of the remaining five (Kehoe et aI., 1991;Levinthal et al, 1985) were published either past our 1985 cutoff date, or in that very year; thus, our quota of 10-15 papers per response system (Lennartz & Weinberger, 1992, p. 97) had been attained. Of the remaining three, one (Kehoe & Gormezano, 1974) was included in the survey; we simply missed the two earliest papers (Levinthal &Papsdorf, 1970, andLevinthal, 1973).…”
Section: Averaging Across Trials Per Sessionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Experiments manipulating a much simpler factor than second-order conditioning have identified a further problem with the assumption that the typical ISI function found in eyeblink conditioning research provides a direct measure of stimulus trace decay. Almost all of the studies described earlier in this section, including those that have provided the main empirical basis for SOP, used massed procedures that involved from 20–100 trials per session and relatively short intertrial intervals, for example, in the range 15–300 s. In a series of studies using a delay conditioning procedure Levinthal and his colleagues showed that, if they gave only one trial per session, a very different ISI function was obtained (e.g., Levinthal, Tartell, Margolin, & Fishman, 1985). In one experiment rates of acquisition of the eyeblink were just as fast with an ISI of 1,100 msec as with one of 200 msec; in a follow up, acquisition was as fast with an ISI of 2,200 msec as with one of 1,200 msec (Levinthal et al, 1985).…”
Section: Eyeblink Conditioning and Wagner’s Sop Theoriesmentioning
confidence: 99%