2016
DOI: 10.1037/xlm0000269
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

The costs and benefits of testing and guessing on recognition memory.

Abstract: We examined whether two types of interpolated tasks (i.e., retrieval-practice via free recall or guessing a missing critical item) improved final recognition for related and unrelated word lists relative to restudying or completing a filler task. Both retrieval-practice and guessing tasks improved correct recognition relative to restudy and filler tasks, particularly when study lists were semantically related. However, both retrieval practice and guessing also generally inflated false recognition for the non-p… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

0
4
0

Year Published

2018
2018
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
5

Relationship

1
4

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(4 citation statements)
references
References 65 publications
0
4
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Evidence for differences in lure detection have been illustrated in two prior studies conducted by the first author. In these studies, participants studied DRM lists (Coane, Huff, & Hutchison, 2016) or categorized lists (Huff et al, 2016) constructed identically to the present lists and were asked to explicitly guess what the critical lure(s) was after study. Participants were more likely to correctly guess DRM lures (.36) than categorized lures (ranging from .20 to .23 across experiments), suggesting that DRM lures are indeed easier for participants to generate and possibly detect on a later test.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Evidence for differences in lure detection have been illustrated in two prior studies conducted by the first author. In these studies, participants studied DRM lists (Coane, Huff, & Hutchison, 2016) or categorized lists (Huff et al, 2016) constructed identically to the present lists and were asked to explicitly guess what the critical lure(s) was after study. Participants were more likely to correctly guess DRM lures (.36) than categorized lures (ranging from .20 to .23 across experiments), suggesting that DRM lures are indeed easier for participants to generate and possibly detect on a later test.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Of these exemplars, the five most common exemplars (i.e., the top five items in the norms) were designated as critical lures and were not studied. The remaining 15 exemplars were then used as study items and presented in descending order of typicality (see Huff, Balota, & Hutchison, 2016;Meade & Roediger, 2006, for a similar procedure). A 260-item recognition test was then constructed and composed of 80 studied list items (from positions 1, 3, 5, 7, 9, 11, 13, and 15 from each studied list), 80 list item controls taken from the nonstudied list set (from the same list positions), 50 critical lures (five per studied list), and 50 critical-lure controls (five per nonstudied list).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Studies that reported data in that manner, as well as studies in which that data could be derived (e.g., number of points earned out of a maximum possible total), were included. For five studies in which a recognition final test was used, proportion correct was used where it was reported as the dependent measure (e.g., Bies-Hernandez, 2014; Huff, Balota, & Hutchison, 2016; Verkoeijen, Bouwmeester, & Camp, 2012) or was derivable from reported mean rates of hits (e.g., Carpenter, 2011) or hits minus false alarms (e.g., Jacoby, Wahlheim, & Coane, 2010). (It should be noted, however, that proportion correct in the case of recognition does not account for response criterion effects and is an incomplete measure of performance; for discussion see Stanislaw & Todorov, 1999.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…With respect to training condition (e.g., testing vs. nontesting reexposure control), each study was coded as using a between- or within-subjects design. In the test-enhanced learning literature, between-subjects designs typically yield larger effect sizes (Rowland, 2014), although that result has not always been obtained when between- versus within-subjects group assignment has been manipulated within a single experiment (e.g., Huff, Balota, & Hutchison, 2016; Rowland, Littrell-Baez, Sensenig, & DeLosh, 2014; Experiment 3; Soderstrom & Bjork, 2014). Intercoder agreement was κ = 0.97.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%